Boy
in a café (S. Yao, Wikicommons)
All
humans were once hunter-gatherers. Back then, versatility came with the
territory. There were only so many game animals, and they differed a lot in
size, shape, and color. So you had to enjoy switching back and forth from one
target animal to another. And you had to enjoy moving from one place to another.
Sooner or later you'd have to.
Beginning
10,000 years ago, farmers made their appearance. Now monotony came with the
territory. A plot of land wasn't something you could forget while you took off
somewhere else. It needed constant care. The tasks were also more repetitive:
ploughing, sowing, harvesting ...
Things
worsened as farming became more advanced. You had to focus on one crop and a
limited number of key tasks.
Different
means of subsistence have selected for different mental traits, and this
selection has had genetic consequences. Monotony avoidance has a heritability
of 0.53 (Saudino, 1999). This predisposition has usually been a handicap in
modern societies, so much so that it often leads to criminality. Males with a
history of early criminal behavior tend to score high on monotony avoidance, as
well as on sensation seeking and low conformity (Klinteberg et al., 1992).
Today,
if you have trouble fitting into your society, you might still survive and
reproduce. In the past, you probably wouldn’t. Other people would take your
place in the gene pool and, over successive generations, their mental makeup would
become the norm.
That’s
gene-culture co-evolution. We have reshaped the world we live in, and this human-made
world has reshaped us. After describing how our ancestors radically changed
their environment, Razib goes on to write: "We were the authors of those
changes, but in the process of telling that story, we became protagonists
within it" (Khan, 2014).
China: a case
study
Advanced
farming—intensive land use, task specialization, monoculture—has profoundly shaped
East Asian societies, particularly China. This is particularly so for rice
farming. Because the paddies need standing water, rice farmers must work
collectively to build, dredge, and drain elaborate irrigation networks. Wheat
farming, by comparison, requires no irrigation and only half as much work.
Advanced
farming seems to have favored a special package of predispositions and
inclinations, including greater acceptance of monotony. This has been shown in
two recent studies.
The
first one was about boredom and how people experience it in their lives. The
results from the 775 Chinese participants were then compared with the results
from a previous survey of 572 Euro-Canadians. It was found that the Chinese
participants were less likely to feel bored in comparable situations. They
seemed to value low-arousal (calm, relaxation) versus high arousal (excitement,
elation) in the case of Euro-Canadians (Ng et al., 2014).
The
authors attributed their findings to cultural learning. One may wonder,
however, why preference for low arousal persists in the face of China’s massive
influx of high-arousal Western culture.
Relational thinking,
collectivism, and favoritism
The
second study had the aim of seeing whether the sociological differences between
rice farmers and wheat farmers have led to differences in mental makeup. When
1,162 Han Chinese performed a series of mental tasks, the results differed
according to whether the participants came from rice-farming regions or
wheat-farming regions (Talhelm et al., 2014).
When
shown a list of three items, such as “train”, “bus”, and “tracks”, and told to choose
two items that pair together, people from rice-farming regions tended to choose
"train and tracks," whereas people from wheat-farming regions tended
to choose "train and bus." The former seemed to be more relational in
their thinking and the latter more abstract. This pattern held up even in
neighboring counties along China's rice-wheat border. People from the rice side
of the border thought more relationally than did people from the wheat side.
A
second task required drawing pictures of yourself and your friends. In a prior
study, Americans drew themselves about 6 mm bigger than they drew their
friends, Europeans drew themselves 3.5 mm bigger, and Japanese drew themselves
slightly smaller. In the present study, people from rice regions were more
likely than people from wheat regions to draw themselves smaller than they drew
their friends. On average, people from wheat regions self-inflated 1.5 mm, and people
from rice regions self-deflated -0.03 mm.
A
third task required imagining yourself doing business with (i) an honest
friend, (ii) a dishonest friend, (iii) an honest stranger, and (iv) a dishonest
stranger. This person might lie, causing you to lose money. Or this person
might be honest, causing you to make money. You could reward or punish this
person accordingly. A previous study found that Singaporeans rewarded friends
much more than they punished them. Americans were much more likely to punish friends
for bad behavior. In this study, people from rice regions were more likely to
remain loyal to friends regardless.
Interestingly,
these findings came from people with no connection to farming at all. They grew
up in a modern urban society, and most were too young to have known the China
that existed before the economic reforms of the late 1970s. It looks like rice regions have favored hardwiring
of certain psychological traits: less abstract thinking and more relational
thinking, less individualism and more collectivism, and less impartiality
toward strangers and more favoritism toward kin and friends.
Why farming sucks,
for you but not for me
These
findings corroborate the ethnographic literature on the differences in
mentality between hunter-gatherers and farmers. Hunter-gatherers typically see
farming as a kind of slavery, and they have trouble understanding well-meaning
outsiders who want to turn them into land-slaves.
Yes,
for the same land area, farming can produce much more food. But it's hard work,
not only physically but mentally as well. Humans had to undergo a change in
mentality before they could make the transition from hunting and gathering to
farming
Those
humans ended up transforming not just their physical landscape but also their
social and cultural landscape … and ultimately themselves. By creating new values
and social relations, they changed the rules for survival and reproduction,
thereby changing the sort of mentality that future generations would inherit.
Humans
transformed the world through farming, and the world returned the favor.
References
Khan,
R. (2014). Our cats, ourselves, The New
York Times, The Opinion Pages, November 24
http://www.nytimes.com/2014/11/25/opinion/our-cats-ourselves.html
Klinteberg,
B., K. Humble, and D. Schalling. (1992). Personality and psychopathy of males
with a history of early criminal behaviour, European
Journal of Personality, 6(4),
245-266.
http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1993-18051-001
Ng,
A.H., Y. Liu, J-Z. Chen, and J.D. Eastwood. (2014). Culture and state boredom:
A comparison between European Canadians and Chinese, Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 13-18.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0191886914006321
Saudino,
K.J., J.R. Gagne, J. Grant, A. Ibatoulina, T. Marytuina, I. Ravich-Scherbo, and
K. Whitfield. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on personality in
adult Russian twins, International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 23,
375-389.
http://jbd.sagepub.com/content/23/2/375.short
Talhelm,
T., X. Zhang, S. Oishi, C. Shimin, D. Duan, X. Lan, and S. Kitayama. (2014).
Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus
wheat agriculture, Science, 344, 603-607.
http://internationalpsychoanalysis.net/wp-content/uploads/2014/05/RiceversusWheatScience-2014-Talhelm-603-8.pdf