In the Reign of
Terror,
by Jessie Macgregor (1891). We don’t respond equally to signs of emotional
distress in other people (Wikicommons)
We
like to think that all people feel empathy to the same degree. In reality, it varies
a lot from one person to the next, like most mental traits. We are half-aware
of this when we distinguish between "normal people" and
"psychopaths," the latter having an abnormally low capacity for
empathy. The distinction is arbitrary, like the one between "tall"
and "short." As with stature, empathy varies continuously among the
individuals of a population, with psychopaths being the ones we find beyond an
arbitrary cut-off point and who probably have many other things wrong with them.
By focusing on the normal/abnormal dichotomy, we lose sight of the variation
that occurs among so-called normal individuals. We probably meet people every
day who have a low capacity for empathy and who nonetheless look and act normal. Because
they seem normal, we assume they are as empathetic as we are. They aren’t.
Like
most mental traits, empathy is heritable, its heritability being estimated at
68% (Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen, 2013). It has two distinct components:
cognitive empathy and affective empathy. Some researchers identify a third
component, pro-social behavior, but its relationship to the other two seems
tangential.
Cognitive
empathy appears to be the evolutionarily older component of the two. It is the
capacity to understand how another person is feeling and then predict how
different actions will affect that person’s emotional state. But this capacity
can be used for selfish purposes. Examples are legion: the con artist; many
telemarketers; the rapist who knows how to charm his victims ...
Affective
empathy is the younger component, having developed out of cognitive empathy. It
is the capacity not just to understand another person's emotional state but
also to identify with it. A person with high affective empathy will try to help
someone in distress not because such help is personally advantageous or legally
required, but because he or she is actually feeling the same distress.
Affective
empathy may have initially evolved as a means to facilitate relations between a
mother and her children. Later, and to varying degrees, it became extended to
other human relationships. This evolutionary trajectory is perceptible in young
children:
Children
do not display empathic concern toward all people equally. Instead, they show
bias toward individuals and members of groups with which they identify. For
instance, young children of 2 years of age display more concern-related
behaviors toward their mother than toward unfamiliar people. Moreover, children
(aged 3-9 years) view social categories as marking patterns of interpersonal
obligations. They view people as responsible only to their own group members,
and consider within-group harm as wrong regardless of explicit rules, but they
view the wrongness of between-group harm as contingent on the presence of such
rules. (Decety and Cowell, 2014)
Similarly,
MRI studies show that adults are much more likely to experience emotional
distress when they see loved ones in pain than when they see strangers in pain.
A stranger in distress will evoke a response only to the degree that the observer
has a high capacity for affective empathy. The higher the capacity the more it will
encompass not only loved ones but also less related individuals, including
total strangers and nonhumans:
Humans
can feel empathic concern for a wide range of 'others', including for nonhuman
animals, such as pets (in the Western culture) or tamagotchi (in Japan). This
is especially the case when signs of vulnerability and need are noticeable. In
support of this, neural regions involved in perceiving the distress of other
humans, such as the anterior cingulate cortex and insula, are similarly activated
when witnessing the distress of domesticated animals (Decety and Cowell, 2014)
While
we associate affective empathy with morality, the two are not the same, and
there are situations where the two come into conflict. In most societies,
kinship is the main organizing principle of social relations, and morality affirms
this principle by spelling out the duties to one's parents, one's kin, and
one's ethny. The importance of kinship may be seen in the Ten Commandments,
which we wrongfully assume to be universal in application. We are told we must
not kill, steal, lie, or commit adultery if the victims are "thy
neighbor," which is explained as meaning "the children of thy
people" (Leviticus 19:18). High-empathy
individuals may thus subvert morality if they view all human distress as being
equal in value. At best, they will neglect loved ones in order to help an
indefinitely large number of needy strangers. At worst, strangers may develop
strategies to exploit high-empathy individuals, i.e., to milk them for all they
are worth.
Mapping empathy in
the human brain
Empathy
appears to arise from specific mechanisms in the brain, and not from a more
general property, like general intelligence. It is produced by a sequence of
mental events, beginning with "mirror neurons" that fire in tandem with
the observed behavior of another person, thereby generating a mental model of
this behavior. Copies of the model are sent elsewhere in the brain to decode
the nature and purpose of the behavior and to predict the sensory consequences
for the observed person. Affective empathy goes further by feeding these
predicted consequences into the observer's emotional state (Carr et al., 2003).
Recent
MRI research has confirmed that empathy is associated with increased
development of certain regions within the brain. Individuals who score high on
cognitive empathy have denser gray matter in the midcingulate cortex and the
adjacent dorsomedial prefontal cortex, whereas individuals who score high on
affective empathy have denser gray matter in the insula cortex (Eres et al.,2015). A high capacity for affective empathy is also associated with a larger
amygdala, which seems to control the way we respond to facial expressions of
fear and other signs of emotional distress (Marsh et al., 2014).
Can
these brain regions be used to measure our capacity for affective empathy? Two
studies, one American and one English, have found that
"conservatives" tend to have a larger right amygdala (Kanai et al.,2011; Schreiber et al., 2013). This has been spun, perhaps predictably, as
proof that the political right is fear-driven (Hibbing et al., 2014). A
likelier explanation is that "conservatives" are disproportionately
drawn from populations that have, on average, a higher capacity for affective
empathy.
Do human
populations vary in their capacity for affective empathy?
Is
it possible, then, that this capacity varies among human populations, just as
it varies among individuals? I have argued that affective empathy is more
adaptive in larger, more complex societies where kinship obligations can no
longer restrain behavior that seriously interferes with the ability of
individuals to live together peacefully and constructively (Frost, 2015).
Whereas affective empathy was originally expressed mainly between a mother and
her children, it has become progressively extended in some populations to a wider
range of interactions. This evolutionary change may be compared to the capacity
to digest milk sugar: initially, this capacity was limited to early childhood,
but in dairy cattle cultures it has become extended into adulthood.
I
have also argued that this evolutionary change has gone the farthest in
Europeans north and west of the Hajnal Line (Frost, 2014a). In these populations,
kinship has been a weaker force in organizing social relations, at least since
the early Middle Ages and perhaps since prehistoric times. There has thus been
selection for mechanisms, like affective empathy, that can regulate social
interaction between unrelated individuals. This selection may have intensified
during two time periods:
-
An initial period corresponding to the emergence of complex
hunter/fisher/gatherers during the Mesolithic along the shores of the North Sea
and the Baltic. Unlike other hunter-gatherers, who were typically small bands
of individuals, these people were able to form large coastal communities by
exploiting abundant marine resources. Such communities were beset, however, by the
problem of enforcing rule compliance on unrelated people, the result being
strong selection for rule-compliant individuals who share certain
predispositions, namely affective empathy, proneness to guilt, and willingness
to obey moral rules and to expel anyone who does not (Frost, 2013a; Frost, 2013b).
-
A second period corresponding to the spread of Christianity among Northwest
Europeans, particularly with the outbreeding, population growth, and increase
in manorialism that followed the Dark Ages (hbd chick, 2014). The result was a
"fruitful encounter" between the two: on the one hand, Christianity,
with its emphasis on internalized morality, struck a responsive chord in these
populations; on the other hand, the latter modified Christianity, increasing
its emphasis on faith, compassion, and original sin (Frost, 2014b).
Conclusion
Recent
research has brought much insight into the nature of empathy, which should no longer
be viewed as being simply a noble precept. We now understand it as the outcome
of a sequence of events in specific regions of the brain. We have also learned
that individuals vary in their capacity for empathy and that most of this
variability is heritable, as is the case with most mental traits. Moreover,
empathy has two components—cognitive and affective—and the strength of one in
relation to the other likewise varies. Although we often consider affective
empathy to be desirable, it can have perverse and even
pathological effects in some contexts.
References
Carr, L., M. Iacoboni, M-C. Dubeau, J.C.
Mazziotta, and G.L. Lenzi. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A
relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 100, 5497-5502.
http://www.ucp.pt/site/resources/documents/ICS/GNC/ArtigosGNC/AlexandreCastroCaldas/7_CaIaDuMaLe03.pdf
Chakrabarti,
B. and S. Baron-Cohen. (2013). Understanding the genetics of empathy and the
autistic spectrum, in S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, M. Lombardo. (eds). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from
Developmental Social Neuroscience, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
http://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=eTdLAAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA326&ots=fHpygaxaMQ&sig=_sJsVgdoe0hc-fFbzaW3GMEslZU#v=onepage&q&f=false
Decety,
J. and J. Cowell. (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 337-339
http://spihub.org/site/resource_files/publications/spi_wp_135_decety.pdf
Eres,
R., J. Decety, W.R. Louis, and P. Molenberghs. (2015). Individual differences
in local gray matter density are associated with differences in affective and
cognitive empathy, NeuroImage, 117, 305-310.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811915004206
Frost,
P. (2013a). The origins of Northwest European guilt culture, Evo and Proud, December 7
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2013/12/the-origins-of-northwest-european-guilt.html
Frost,
P. (2013b). Origins of Northwest European guilt culture, Part II, Evo and Proud, December 14
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2013/12/origins-of-northwest-european-guilt.html
Frost,
P. (2014a). Compliance with Moral Norms: a Partly Heritable Trait? Evo and Proud, April 12
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2014/04/compliance-with-moral-norms-partly.html
Frost,
P. (2014b). A fruitful encounter, Evo and
Proud, September 26
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2014/09/a-fruitful-encounter.html
Frost,
P. (2015). Two paths, The Unz Review,
January 24
http://www.unz.com/pfrost/two-paths/
hbd
chick (2014). Medieval manorialism’s selection pressures, hbd chick, November 19
https://hbdchick.wordpress.com/2014/11/19/medieval-manorialisms-selection-pressures/
Hibbing,
J.R., K.B. Smith, and J.R. Alford. (2014). Differences in negativity bias
underlie variations in political ideology, Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 37, 297-350
http://www.geoffreywetherell.com/Hibbing%20et%20al%20paper%20and%20commentaries%20(1).pdf
Kanai,
R., T. Feilden, C. Firth, and G. Rees. (2011). Political orientations are
correlated with brain structure in young adults, Current Biology, 21, 677
- 680.
http://www.cell.com/current-biology/abstract/S0960-9822(11)00289-2
Keysers,
C. and V. Gazzola. (2014). Dissociating the ability and propensity for empathy,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 163-166.
http://www.cell.com/trends/cognitive-sciences/pdf/S1364-6613(13)00296-9.pdf
Marsh,
A.A., S.A. Stoycos, K.M. Brethel-Haurwitz, P. Robinson, J.W. VanMeter, and E.M.
Cardinale. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary
altruists, Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 111,
15036-15041.
http://www.pnas.org/content/111/42/15036.short
Schreiber,
D., Fonzo, G., Simmons, A.N., Dawes, C.T., Flagan, T., et al. (2013). Red
Brain, Blue Brain: Evaluative Processes Differ in Democrats and Republicans. PLoS ONE 8(2): e52970.
http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0052970
Empathy is actually a trait of liberals. I don't understand your claim that "'conservatives' are disproportionately drawn from populations that have, on average, a higher capacity for affective empathy." It's the opposite. Conservatives are those who refuse to feel or relate to the situation of groups like minorities, which is why they are against expanding rights and freedoms. Instead they're driven by justice, duty, discipline, and some other traits.
ReplyDeleteExcellent. You really know how to write -- and research.
ReplyDeleteAbout liberals vs. conservatives on affective empathy, I have a conservative girl friend who cares passionately about animal abuse. One data point.
ReplyDeleteAbout liberals vs. conservatives on affective empathy, I have a conservative girl friend who cares passionately about animal abuse. One data point.
ReplyDeleteI think most recreational hunters are conservatives. The point of recreational hunting is to derive pleasure and enjoyment from the activity of violently killing animals. I don't think these or the "I have a conservative friend who cares about animal abuse" are really data points though. I don't think the former are necessarily examples of less affective empathy, and the latter of more.
If denser population tends to be selective for affective empathy, then how about ancient Near East and Egypt for example? Or Indus and ancient China?
ReplyDeleteAll of those had high population densities for millenia as far as I know.
"Empathy is actually a trait of liberals. I don't understand your claim that "'conservatives' are disproportionately drawn from populations that have, on average, a higher capacity for affective empathy." It's the opposite. Conservatives are those who refuse to feel or relate to the situation of groups like minorities, which is why they are against expanding rights and freedoms. Instead they're driven by justice, duty, discipline, and some other traits."
ReplyDeleteThe data suggest that at least in the United States, white liberals and white conservatives both have similar amounts of empathy. The difference is that white conservatives have similarly strong feelings of loyalty, sanctity, and respect for authority. In contrast, white liberals tend to view loyalty to one's family, country and race as being morally suspect, for example.
Reader,
ReplyDeleteTerms like "liberal" and "conservative" are problematic. There has always been a dimension of tribal politics in those terms, and that tendency seems to be getting stronger. If I were a cynic, I could say that liberals believe so much in compassion that they want the government to do it for them. This argument was actually made back in the 1960s: individual acts of compassion are ineffectual and create an unfair patchwork of social assistance. Compassion must be "nationalized" so that it can be delivered fairly and equally to everyone.
Luke,
I'm not sure what I am. I honestly like most people who identify as "liberals" but likeable people can still make mistakes, often costly ones.
Anon,
As a kid, I knew many hunters. I respect hunters who eat the game animals they kill, and I also respect those that kill animals (like deer) that become too numerous and overgraze their habitat. But killing for pleasure has always seemed wrong to me. I've never hunted in my life.
Juoni,
It's not population density so much as being in a community where most of the people you interact with are nonkin. In India, China, and the Middle East, people tend to congregate with and interact with close kin. In the case of complex hunter-gatherers, like those of the Mesolithic, people congregated on a seasonal basis. They would disperse inland during the winter for hunting and then congregate in large coastal settlements for fishing. This kind of community is difficult to segregate into kin-based neighborhoods, as in a normal urban community. Relations with nonkin have to be developed and made reliable within a short span of time because of the need to coordinate access to the sea and to the limited shoreline space.
Sid,
In both the U.S. and English study, the data were not broken down by race or ethnicity. The U.S. study was done at the University of California, San Diego, where most "liberals" are probably Hispanic or Asian.