Hadza
men are smaller, less robust, and less aggressive than the more polygynous
Datoga (Wikicommons - Idobi).
Humans
differ in paternal investment—the degree to which fathers help mothers care for
their offspring. They differ in this way between individuals, between populations, and
between stages of cultural evolution.
During
the earliest stage, when all humans were hunter-gatherers, men invested more in
their offspring with increasing distance from the equator. Longer, colder
winters made it harder for women to gather food for themselves and their
children. They had to rely on meat from their hunting spouses. Conversely,
paternal investment was lower in the tropics, where women could gather food
year-round and provide for themselves and their children with little male
assistance.
This
sexual division of labor influenced the transition to farming. In the tropics,
women were the main providers for their families as gatherers of fruits,
berries, roots, and other wild plant foods. They were the ones who developed
farming, thereby biasing it toward domestication of wild plants.
This
may be seen in sub-Saharan Africa, where farming arose near the Niger's
headwaters and gave rise to the Sudanic food complex—a wide range of native
crops now found throughout the continent (sorghum, pearl millet, cow pea, etc.)
and only one form of livestock, the guinea fowl (Murdock, 1959, pp. 44, 64-68).
Many wild animal species could have been domesticated for meat production, but
women were much less familiar with them. Men knew these species as hunters but
had little motivation to domesticate them. Why should they? Women were the main
providers.
And
so women shouldered even more the burden of providing for themselves and their
offspring. Men in turn found it easier to go back on the mate market and get
second or third wives. Finally, men had to compete against each other much more
for fewer unmated women.
There
was thus a causal chain: female dominance of farming => female reproductive
autonomy => male polygyny => male-male rivalry for access to women. Jack
Goody (1973) in his review of the literature says: "The desire of men to
attract wives is seen as correlated with the degree of women's participation in
the basic productive process." The more women produce, the lower the cost
of polygyny.
In
sub-Saharan Africa, the cost was often negative. Goody quotes a 17th century
traveler on the Gold Coast: the women till the ground "whilst the man only
idly spends his time in impertinent tattling (the woman's business in our
country) and drinking of palm-wine, which the poor wives are frequently obliged
to raise money to pay for, and by their hard labour maintain and satisfie these
lazy wretches their greedy thirst after wines."
Goody
cites data from southern Africa showing that the polygyny rate fell when the
cost of polygyny rose:
In
Basutoland one in nine husbands had more than one wife in 1936; in 1912, it was
one in 5.5 (Mair 1953: 10). Hunter calculates that in 1911 12 per cent of Pondo
men were plurally married and the figure was slightly lower in 1921. In 1946,
the Tswana rate was 11 per cent; according to a small sample collected by
Livingstone in 1850 it was 43 per cent. The figures appear to have changed
drastically over time and the reasons are interesting. 'The large household is
now not a source of wealth, but a burden which only the rich can bear' (Mair
1953: 19). Not only is there a specific tax for each additional wife, but a
man's wives now no longer give the same help in agriculture that they did
before. One reason for this is that the fields are ploughed rather than hoed.
Among the Pondo, 'the use of the plough means that the amount of grain
cultivated no longer depends on women's labour' (Goody, 1973)
Although
polygynous marriage has become less common in southern Africa, polygynous
behavior seems as frequent as ever. To a large degree, polygynous marriage has
given way to more transient forms of polygyny: prostitution and other informal
arrangements.
Goody also notes that women are much less self-reliant in the northern savannah of West Africa:
In savannah regions where water is scarce and trees scattered, their collection may make great demands on a woman’s time. So too does the grinding of hard grain, in the absence of mills. In all these domestic pursuits the savannah is more demanding on a woman’s time than the forest and consequently she can often make less contribution to agriculture. (Goody, 1973)
Yet polygyny rates have remained high. Goody gives the example of Ghana. Polygyny rates are about the same in the north and the south, yet in the north men participate much more in farming.
So what is going on? Goody concludes that "female farming and polygyny are clearly associated in a general way" but ultimately the "reasons behind polygyny are sexual and reproductive rather than economic and productive." It would be more parsimonious to say that the polygyny rate increases when the cost of providing for a woman and her children decreases for men. Over time, low-cost polygyny selects for men who are more motivated to exploit sexual opportunities. This new mindset influences the subsequent course of gene-culture coevolution.
Goody also notes that women are much less self-reliant in the northern savannah of West Africa:
In savannah regions where water is scarce and trees scattered, their collection may make great demands on a woman’s time. So too does the grinding of hard grain, in the absence of mills. In all these domestic pursuits the savannah is more demanding on a woman’s time than the forest and consequently she can often make less contribution to agriculture. (Goody, 1973)
Yet polygyny rates have remained high. Goody gives the example of Ghana. Polygyny rates are about the same in the north and the south, yet in the north men participate much more in farming.
So what is going on? Goody concludes that "female farming and polygyny are clearly associated in a general way" but ultimately the "reasons behind polygyny are sexual and reproductive rather than economic and productive." It would be more parsimonious to say that the polygyny rate increases when the cost of providing for a woman and her children decreases for men. Over time, low-cost polygyny selects for men who are more motivated to exploit sexual opportunities. This new mindset influences the subsequent course of gene-culture coevolution.
Such
gene-culture coevolution has gone through four stages in the evolutionary history
of sub-Saharan Africans:
First stage
Tropical
hunter-gatherers were already oriented toward low paternal investment. Men had
a lesser role in child rearing because year-round food gathering provided women
with a high degree of food autonomy. Women were thus selected for self-reliance
and men for polygyny. Pair bonding was correspondingly weak in both sexes.
Second stage
This
mindset guided tropical hunter-gatherers in their transition to farming. In
short, female-dominated food gathering gave way to female-dominated
horticulture—hoe farming of various crops with almost no livestock raising.
Women became even more autonomous, and men even more polygynous. There was thus
further selection for a mindset of female self-reliance, male polygyny, and
weak pair bonding.
Third stage
A
similar process occurred with the development of trade. Female-dominated
horticulture tended to orient women, much more than men, toward the market
economy. This has particularly been so in West Africa, where markets are
overwhelmingly run by women. Trade has thus become another means by which
African women provide for themselves and their children.
Fourth stage
Female-dominated
horticulture has given way to male-dominated farming (pastoralism, cereal crops) in some regions, such
as the northern savannah regions of West Africa. Despite higher male participation in farming, the pre-existing
mindset has tended to maintain high polygyny rates. We see a similar tendency
in southern Africa, where polygyny rates have fallen over the past century, and
yet polygynous behavior persists in the form of prostitution and less formal
sexual arrangements.
The Hadza and the
Datoga
Mode
of subsistence, mating system, and mindset are thus interrelated. These
interrelationships are discussed by Butovskaya et al. (2015) in their study of
two peoples in Tanzania: the largely monogamous Hadza (hunter-gatherers) and
the highly polygynous Datoga (pastoralists). In their review of previous
studies, the authors note:
In
hunter-gatherer societies, such as the monogamous Hadza of Tanzania (Africa),
men invest more in offspring than in small-scale pastoralist societies, such as
the polygynous Datoga of Tanzania [12-14]. Polygyny and between-group
aggression redirect men's efforts from childcare toward investment in male-male
relationships and the pursuit of additional mates [15]. When men participate in
childcare, their testosterone (T) level decreases [15-18]. Muller et al. [19]
found that, among the monogamous, high paternally investing Hadza, T levels
were lower for fathers than for non-fathers. This effect was not observed among
the polygynous, low paternally investing Datoga. (Butovskaya et al., 2015).
Butovskaya
et al. (2015) confirmed these previous findings in their own study:
Datoga
males reported greater aggression than Hadza men—a finding in line with
previous reports [29,30]. It is important to mention several striking
differences between these two cultures. There is a negative attitude toward
aggression among the Hadza but not among the Datoga. In situations of potential
aggression, the Hadza prefer to leave [30]. In contrast, aggression is an
instrument of social control—both within the family and in outgroup relations
in Datoga society. Datoga men are trained to compete with each other and to act
aggressively in particular circumstances [30]
The
authors also confirmed differences in reproductive behavior between the two
groups:
Our
research indicates a difference in the number of children in Hadza and Datoga
men achieved after the age of 50. This may be interpreted as differences
attributable to different life trajectories and marriage patterns. Beginning in
early childhood, boys in the two societies are subjected to different social
and environmental pressures (e.g., it is typical for Datoga parents to punish
children for misbehavior, while parental violence is much less typical for Hadza
parents). Hadza men start reproducing in the early 20s, but their reproductive
success later in life is associated with their hunting skills [15]. In the
Datoga, men marry later, typically in their 30s. Male status and, consequently,
social and reproductive success in the Datoga are positively correlated with
fighting abilities and risk-taking in raiding expeditions among younger men,
and with wealth, dominance, and social skills among older men. In the Datoga,
as in other patrilineal societies, fathers do not invest directly in child
care, but children do benefit from their father's investment in the form of
wealth and social protection, as well as various services provided by father's
patrilineal male relatives [56]. In polygynous societies, spending resources on
attracting additional wives may be more beneficial [40,57,58]. It would be
difficult for some men to invest directly in providing for all their children,
given that men with multiple wives can father a considerable number of
children, and that households with wives may be located at substantial distance
from one another.
This
behavioral difference seems to be mediated by differing levels of androgens,
such as testosterone:
The
effect of androgens, such as T, operates through stimulation of androgen
receptors [21-23]. The androgen receptor (AR) gene contains a polymorphic and
functional locus in exon 1, comprising two triplets (CAG and GGN). This locus
supports a regulatory function that responds to T, with fewer CAG repeat
clusters being more effective in transmitting the T signal [22]. Moreover, the
length of the GGN repeat predicts circulating and free T in men.
At
the androgen receptor gene, the authors found fewer CAG repeats in the Datoga
than in the Hadza. The number of repeats was also more variable in the Datoga.
The Datoga's higher and more variable polygyny rates thus seem to correlate
with higher and more variable levels of testosterone.
The
authors also wished to see whether these differing levels of testosterone
correlate with differing levels of aggressiveness. To this end, they
interviewed the Hadza and Datoga participants:
They
were asked to provide information including their age, sex, marital status,
number of children, ethnicity and aggression history (especially fights with
other tribal members). All questions were read aloud in one-to-one dialogues
and further explanations were provided, if necessary. Self-reported aggression
was assessed with the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ; [48]). The
BPAQ includes 29 statements, grouped into four subscales—physical aggression (9
items), verbal aggression (5 items), anger (7 items), and hostility (8 items)—answered
on aLikert scale anchored by 1 (extremely uncharacteristic of me) and 5
(extremely characteristic of me).
Total
aggression was found to correlate negatively with CAG repeat number. Age group
did not predict aggression.
More polygyny =
stronger sexual selection of men
Finally,
the authors suggest that Datoga men, with their higher polygyny rate and
fiercer competition for access to women, have undergone greater sexual
selection. They have thus become bigger and more masculine than Hadza men.
Although this selection pressure also exists among the Hadza, the driving force
of sexual selection has been weaker because Hadza men are more monogamous and
less sexually competitive:
Our
findings are in concordance with other research, demonstrating that even among
the relatively egalitarian Hadza there is selection pressure in favor of more
masculine men [59-62]. At the same time, preference for more masculine
partners, with greater height and body size, is culturally variable and
influenced by the degree of polygyny, local ecology, and other economic and
social factors [59-62]. Many Datoga women commented that they would like to
avoid taller and larger men as marriage partners, as they may be dangerously
violent [44,62]. Only 2% of Hadza women listed large body size as an attractive
mate characteristic [63]. Hadza marriages in which the wife is taller than the
husband are common, and as frequent as would be expected by chance [64]. (Butovskaya et al., 2015)
This
is consistent with what we see in nonhuman polygynous species. Successful males
tend to be the ones that are better not only at attracting the opposite sex but
also at fighting off rivals. They thus become bigger, tougher, and meaner.
This
is also consistent with what we see generally in the highly polygynous farming
peoples of sub-Saharan Africa. They and their African-American descendants
exceed European-descended subjects in weight, chest size, arm girth, leg girth,
muscle fiber properties, and bone density (Ama et al., 1986; Ettinger et al.,1997; Himes, 1988; Hui et al., 2003; Pollitzer and Anderson, 1989; Todd and Lindala, 1928; Wagner and Heyward, 2000; Wolff and Steggerda, 1943; Wright et al., 1995).
References
Ama,
P.F.M., J.A. Simoneau, M.R. Boulay, O. Serresse, G. Thériault, and C. Bouchard.
(1986). Skeletal muscle characteristics in sedentary Black and Caucasian males,
Journal of Applied Physiology, 61, 1758-1761.
http://www.educadorfisicoadinis.com.br/download/artigos/Skeletal%20muscle%20characteristics%20in%20sedentary%20black%20and%20Caucasian%20males.pdf
Butovskaya
M.L., O.E. Lazebny, V.A. Vasilyev, D.A. Dronova, D.V. Karelin, A.Z.P. Mabulla,
et al. (2015). Androgen receptor gene polymorphism, aggression, and
reproduction in Tanzanian foragers and pastoralists. PLoS ONE 10(8): e0136208.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/281170838_Androgen_Receptor_Gene_Polymorphism_Aggression_and_Reproduction_in_Tanzanian_Foragers_and_Pastoralists
Ettinger,
B., S. Sidney, S.R. Cummings, C. Libanati, D.D. Bikle, I.S. Tekawa, K. Tolan,
and P. Steiger. (1997). Racial differences in bone density between young adult
black and white subjects persist after adjustment for anthropometric, lifestyle,
and biochemical differences, Journal of
Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 82, 429-434.
http://press.endocrine.org/doi/abs/10.1210/jcem.82.2.3732
Goody,
J. (1973). Polygyny, economy and the role of women, in J. Goody (ed.) The Character of Kinship, Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
https://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=TFjf4mUHqv4C&oi=fnd&pg=PA175&dq=Polygyny,+economy+and+the+role+of+women&ots=Lf9w6wxY9D&sig=3d0RBnoMbGUd2OYqghzhwdsO3BA#v=onepage&q&f=false
Himes,
J. H. (1988). Racial variation in physique and body composition, Canadian Journal of Sport
Sciences,
13, 117-126.
http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/3293730
Hui,
S.L., L.A. DiMeglio, C. Longcope, M. Peacock, R. McClintock, A.J. Perkins, and
C.C. Johnston Jr. (2003). Difference in bone mass between Black and White
American children: Attributable to body build, sex hormone levels, or bone
turnover? Journal of Clinical
Endocrinology & Metabolism, 88,
642-649.
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Conrad_Johnston/publication/10911942_Difference_in_bone_mass_between_black_and_white_American_children_attributable_to_body_build_sex_hormone_levels_or_bone_turnover/links/548f36af0cf225bf66a7fdc1.pdf
Murdock,
G.P. (1959). Africa. Its Peoples and
Their Culture History, New York: McGraw-Hill.
Pollitzer,
W.S. and J.J. Anderson. (1989). Ethnic and genetic differences in bone mass: a
review with a hereditary vs environmental perspective, American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 50, 1244-1259
http://ajcn.nutrition.org/content/50/6/1244.short
Todd,
T.W., and A. Lindala. (1928). Dimensions of the body: Whites and American
Negroes of both sexes, American Journal
of Physical Anthropology, 12,
35-101.
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajpa.1330120104/abstract
Wagner,
D.R. and V.H. Heyward. (2000). Measures of body composition in blacks and
whites: a comparative review, American
Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 71,
1392-1402.
http://ajcn.nutrition.org/content/71/6/1392.short
Wolff,
G. and M. Steggerda. (1943). Female-male index of body build in Negroes and
Whites: An interpretation of anatomical sex differences, Human Biology, 15,
127-152.
Wright,
N.M., J. Renault, S. Willi, J.D. Veldhuis, J.P. Pandey, L. Gordon, L.L. Key,
and N.H. Bell. (1995). Greater secretion of growth hormone in black than in
white men: possible factor in greater bone mineral density-a clinical research
center study, Journal of Clinical
Endocrinology & Metabolism, 80,
2291-2297.
http://press.endocrine.org/doi/abs/10.1210/jcem.80.8.7543111
You will become a pariah for such racist statements.
ReplyDeleteTruth is not a defense.
It is interesting that in parts of Southern China and South East Asia both women and men work hard in the fields.
ReplyDeleteI also wonder if we can make a case that civilization depends on men being willing to work hard.
Were there many potential native African species of wildlife eligible for domestication? Jared Diamond said no, and gave reasons.
ReplyDeleteApart from this, how do the Australian Aborigines fit into the equation? They were hunter gatherers in a tropical setting - although the highest populations were in the temperate southeast. Polygyny was the norm, and the largest numbers of wives tended to be in the desert areas, where subsistence was on a razor edge. Typically, the older men monopolized the women; a man got married for the first time about 20 years later than a woman. When he died, his wives were recycled among other men. At the same time, resources were pooled, with the parts of the prey animals being divided up according to strict kinship rules, so that everybody received a share. Also, Aboriginal society was very violent. (It still is.)
The south of Europe is generally considered more polygynous than the north yet Northern Europeans are taller and weigh more than Southern Europeans despite equal levels of nutrition/civilization?
ReplyDeleteCerebral,
ReplyDeleteIt's not so much that monogamous men work harder than polygynous men, but rather that the latter work harder at things that are detrimental to social cohesion, i.e., male-male competition for mates. State formation is thus impeded, as is the creation of larger and more complex societies.
Malcolm,
"Jared Diamond, in discussing animal domestication, claims that the local availability of species with the right qualities for domestication was key, rather than anything special about the biology or culture of the humans living there. In some cases that may be true: there aren’t many large mammals left in Australia, and they’re all marsupials anyway. Stupid marsupials. He claims that since Africans and Amerindians were happy to adopt Eurasian domesticated animals when they became available, it must be that that suitable local animals just didn’t exist. But that’s a non sequitur: making use of an already-domesticated species is not at all the same thing as the original act of domestication. That’s like equating using a cell phone with inventing one. He also says that people have had only mixed success in recent domestication attempts – but the big problem there is that a newly domesticated species doesn’t just have to be good, it has to be better than already-existing domestic animals.
Indian elephants, although not truly domesticated, are routinely tamed and used for work in Southern Asia. The locals in Sub-Saharan Africa seem never to have done this with African elephants – but it is possible. The Belgians, in the Congo, hired Indian mahouts to tame African elephants, with success. It’s still done in the Congo, on a very limited scale, and elephants have recently been tamed in other parts of sub-Saharan Africa, such as the Okavango delta. Elephants have long generations, which makes true domestication difficult, but people have made domestication attempts with eland, African buffalo, and oryx. They’re all tameable, and eland have actually been domesticated to some extent. If a species is tameable, economically useful upon taming, and has a reasonable reproductive schedule, domestication is possible: selection for even a few generations can change their behavior enough to make dealing with them a lot easier."
https://westhunt.wordpress.com/2012/02/15/diamond-on-domestication/
Chris,
Testosterone levels are also higher in southern Europe than in northern Europe, which seems paradoxical. I suspect that stature has decreased in southern Europe over the past three thousand years, perhaps because short stature has some kind of adaptive value in advanced societies.
It's not so much that monogamous men work harder than polygynous men, but rather that the latter work harder at things that are detrimental to social cohesion, i.e., male-male competition for mates. State formation is thus impeded, as is the creation of larger and more complex societies.
ReplyDeleteYes, you are correct.
All men work on those things that increase reproductive success ...
Again with the myth of the "short southern european". Seriously? Italy, Spain are of equal height to Americans and Greece/Balkans is taller than many Northern countries. Every northwestern county is NOT Netherlands. Britain is not that tall. Last time I checked according to European statistics they're below France now. Sweden likewise is not as tall as Netherlands. You guys are taking such an insignificant difference in height and blowing it way out of proportion.
ReplyDelete