RĂªverie, Adrien de Witte
(1850-1935), Wikicommons
African
Americans sleep on average almost an hour less than do Euro Americans. The two
groups have mean sleep times of 6.05 hours and 6.85 hours. This finding has
recently been discussed by Brian Resnick in National
Journal and by our Steve Sailer.
Researchers
reject a genetic explanation: "There is a consensus that innate biological
differences between blacks and whites are not a factor" (Resnick, 2015). So
what is the cause?
One
study points the finger at racism:
"If you can take out that discrimination piece, the average
African-American and the average Caucasian look at lot more similar. [...] "It's not perfect, but in terms of
sleep, a lot of the disparity goes away" (Resnick, 2015).
The
study is by Tomfohr et al. (2012). It found that duration of deep sleep and
duration of Stage 2 of light sleep correlated in African Americans with perceived
discrimination, which is defined as "the extent to which an individual
believes that members of his or her ethnic group have been discriminated
against in society."
Nonetheless,
as the authors note, sleep duration still differs significantly between African
and Euro Americans even when the difference is adjusted for the effects of
perceived discrimination. So we are left with a curious finding: two separate
causes, one genetic and the other environmental, are producing the same pattern
of effects. Both are reducing deep sleep and Stage 2 light sleep in African
Americans while not affecting Stage 1 light sleep.
Whenever
I see this kind of finding, I start looking for confounds. Is one cause a sock
puppet for the other? It may be that perceived discrimination increases with
African ancestry. Perhaps African Americans who feel conscious of
discrimination also tend to be darker-skinned and more visibly African than
those who don’t. This confound has actually been shown by several studies, such
as the following:
This
study tested the extent to which skin color is associated with differential
exposure to discrimination for a sample of 300 Black adults. Results revealed
that dark-skinned Blacks were 11 times more likely to experience frequent
racial discrimination than their light-skinned counterparts; 67% of subjects
reporting high discrimination were dark-skinned and only 8.5% were
light-skinned. (Klonoff and Landrine, 2000; see also Keith and Herring, 1991)
Even
if perceived discrimination could fully explain the race difference in sleep
duration, we still couldn’t exclude a genetic explanation, since the degree of
perceived discrimination is confounded with the degree of African ancestry.
In
reality, perceived discrimination accounts for only part of the race
difference, and since this difference remains significant even if we factor out
that putative cause, the most parsimonious explanation is a genetic cause. Only
that cause can fully account for shorter sleep duration in African Americans.
Studies in Africa
Another
way to solve this puzzle is to look at Africans living in Africa. Do they show
the same pattern we see in African Americans?
We
know less about sleep patterns in Africa, but what we do know suggests that
Africans, too, have shorter sleep duration. When Friborg et al. (2012) studied
sleep in Ghanaians and Norwegians, they found that Ghanaians slept about an hour
less than do Norwegians on weekends and between a quarter and half an hour less
on weekdays. Oluwole (2010) studied sleep in Nigerian undergraduates and found
they slept an average of 6.2 hours plus another 70 minutes in the afternoon.
This pattern is actually typical in the tropical zone. People prefer to get
some sleep when the temperature is at its peak and spend more time awake when
it's more bearable.
But
why would this pattern persist in African Americans? Perhaps it’s hardwired to
some degree. When siestas become the cultural norm, there is selection for
those individuals who enjoy being normal (and against those who don't).
Sleep
patterns are heritable:
Assessed
self-reported sleep data from 2,238 monozygotic (MZ) and 4,545 dizygotic (DZ)
adult twin pairs born in Finland before 1958. Results indicate a significant
hereditary effect on sleep length and on sleep quality. When the data were
examined in subgroups defined by sex, age (18-24 yrs and 25+ yrs), and
cohabitation status of the twin pair, the highest heritability estimates for
sleep length were for Ss living together aged 25 yrs or older. For Ss living
apart, the heritability estimates were statistically significant in all Ss aged
25 yrs or older. For sleep quality, significant heritability estimates were
found for all groups except women living together. Results indicate that a
significant proportion of the variance in sleep length and quality was due to
factors that make MZ Ss more similar than DZ Ss. (Partinen et al., 1983)
A
single genetic polymorphism seems to explain much of the variability between
individuals in sleep patterns, particularly deep sleep and slow wave activity
(SWA):
Here
we show in humans that a genetic variant of adenosine deaminase, which is
associated with the reduced metabolism of adenosine to inosine, specifically
enhances deep sleep and SWA during sleep. In contrast, a distinct polymorphism
of the adenosine A2A receptor gene, which was associated with interindividual
differences in anxiety symptoms after caffeine intake in healthy volunteers,
affects the electroencephalogram during sleep and wakefulness in a
non-state-specific manner. Our findings indicate a direct role of adenosine in
human sleep homeostasis. Moreover, our data suggest that genetic variability in
the adenosinergic system contributes to the interindividual variability in
brain electrical activity during sleep and wakefulness. (Retey et al., 2005)
Conclusion
So
African Americans are getting enough sleep at night. It’s just that they're not
getting enough afternoon naps. But aren't naps for kids? Or old fogeys? Actually,
they’re quite normal for adults in much of the world. In the Nigerian study,
82% of the participants regularly took afternoon naps.
It’s
ironic that the “r word” has been injected into this debate. If a behavior
deviates from the white American norm, and if racism is held responsible either
directly or indirectly, one is assuming that this deviation is pathological. It
is “deviant.” It shouldn’t exist and something should be done about it. The
white American norm thus becomes a norm for all humans, and all humans—if they
want to be fully human—should strive toward it.
In
reality, there is no single human nature. Genetic evolution didn’t slow down
when humans began to split up and settle the different continents. It accelerated.
And not just because our ancestors were adapting to different natural
environments. Most of the acceleration took place long after the globe had been
settled from the equator to the arctic. It happened when humans began to adapt
to an increasingly diverse range of cultural environments. And those adaptations
were mostly behavioral and psychological.
One
of them is the way we sleep. The African sleep pattern is normal in its native
environment. It is simply an adaptation to a particular set of circumstances,
just as the northern European sleep pattern is an adaptation to another set of
circumstances.
References
Friborg,
O., B. Bjorvatn, B. Amponsah, and S. Pallesen. (2012), Associations between
seasonal variations in day length (photoperiod), sleep timing, sleep quality
and mood: a comparison between Ghana (5°) and Norway (69°). Journal of Sleep
Research, 21: 176-184.
Keith,
V. M., and Herring, C. (1991). Skin Tone and Stratification in the
Black-Community. American Journal of Sociology, 97(3), 760-778.
Klonoff,
E. A., and Landrine, H. (2000). Is skin color a marker for racial
discrimination? Explaining the skin color-hypertension relationship. Journal of
Behavioural Medicine, 23(4), 329-338.
Oluwole,
O. S. A. (2010), Sleep habits in Nigerian undergraduates. Acta Neurologica
Scandinavica, 121, 1-6.
Partinen,
M., J. Kaprio, M. Koskenvuo, P. Putkonen, and H. Langinvainio (1983). Genetic
and environmental determination of human sleep. Sleep: Journal of Sleep
Research & Sleep Medicine, 6(3), 79-185.
Resnick,
B. (2015). The Black-White sleep gap, National Journal, October 23
Retey,
J.V., M. Adam, E. Honegger, R. Khatami, U.F.O. Luhmann, H.H. Jung, W. Berger,
and H.P. Landolt. (2005). A functional genetic variation of adenosine deaminase
affects the duration and intensity of deep sleep in humans, Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 102, 15676-15681
Sailer,
S. (2015). Racism never sleeps: "The Black-White Sleep Gap: An Unexpected
Challenge in the Quest for Racial Justice", The Unz Review, October 29
Tomfohr,
L., M.A. Pung, K.M. Edwards, and J.E. Dimsdale. (2012). Racial differences in
sleep architecture: The role of ethnic discrimination, Biological Psychology, 89, 34-38.
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Lianne_Tomfohr/publication/51649994_Racial_differences_in_sleep_architecture_The_role_of_ethnic_discrimination/links/00b495314ab6f01fae000000.pdf
I wonder if sleep duration is correlated with IQ suggesting that if your brain is working hard you want more sleep. Just a wild guess.
ReplyDelete
ReplyDeleteDifferent species have different patterns of sleeping
http://mybroadband.co.za/vb/showthread.php/640594-Gestationally-Speaking-Blacks-And-Whites-Are-Distinct-Species
https://books.google.co.za/books?id=q_Cp_fURG28C&pg=PA70&lpg=PA70&dq=different+species+different+sleeping+pattern&source=bl&ots=nisCYJ_tHq&sig=I-N-tnWxPREGM9nL7NGocUJN2us&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwiTysHC-OnJAhVB1RoKHYjQCg4Q6AEIWjAJ#v=onepage&q=different%20species%20different%20sleeping%20pattern&f=false
Sorry, this one doesn't fly because Northern Europeans used to nap in the summers, too, and have biphasic sleep in the winters. They would wake at sunrise even if they'd only gotten four or five hours of sleep and make up part of the deficit after the big afternoon meal, between the heavy labor of the day and the light labor.
ReplyDelete