The
Babylonian goddess Ishtar (Louvre). The cult of feminine whiteness reached its
height in a zone stretching from the Mediterranean through the Middle East and
into South and East Asia
Skin
color differentiates between the sexes at puberty, with complexions becoming
paler in girls and ruddier and browner in boys. The cause seems to be hormonal.
This is most convincingly shown by recent studies on digit ratio in men and
women before and after puberty and by earlier studies on normal, castrated, and
ovariectomized subjects (Edwards and Duntley 1939; Edwards and Duntley 1949;
Edwards et al. 1941; Manning et al. 2004; Sitek et al. 2018).
How noticeable is this sex difference? If we take the best controlled studies,
where this sexual differentiation is measured at the upper inner arm, we find
that boys and girls differ after puberty by about one and a half percentage
points of skin reflectance (Kalla 1983; Mesa 1983). By comparison, northern
Europeans and West Africans differ by 25 to 30 percentage points (Robins 1991,
Tables 7.1, 7.2).
Keep
in mind that these measurements are from the upper inner arm and that
measurements from other body sites show a much larger sex difference. On the
buttocks and the breasts, skin reflectance differs by 6 to 15 percentage points
between men and women (Edwards et al. 1939; Garn et al. 1956). The literature
has always ascribed this finding to differences in dress and, hence, to differences
in sun exposure, yet female skin is lighter at these sites not only because it
has less melanin but also because it has less blood in its outer layers, a fact
hard to reconcile with a simple tanning effect.
There
is reason to believe that women are lighter-skinned where their subcutaneous
fat layer is thicker, possibly because body fat contains an enzyme (aromatase)
that converts an androgen (androstenedione) into an estrogen (estrone), thus
feminizing the skin (Siiteri and MacDonald 1973). Indeed, lightness of skin
color correlates with thickness of subcutaneous fat in adult women (Mazess
1967).
Visual processing
of this sex difference in the human mind
Whatever
its actual magnitude, we seem innately predisposed to notice this small
difference in skin color, particularly for face recognition. Research is
ongoing, but there is a growing consensus that "color is not merely an
accessory of faces, but is rather a complex and crucial feature in facial
processing. [...] Further, recent work has revealed consistent patterns of
connected face and color selective cortical areas, possibly reflecting a shared
overlap of visual processing between faces and color" (Thorstenson 2018).
Faces
are recognized by means of many neurons, some of which specialize in
recognizing male faces, others in recognizing female faces, and others in
recognizing both indifferently (Baudouin and Brochard 2011; Bestelmeyer et al.
2008; Jacquet and Rhodes 2008; Little et al. 2005). To recognize gender, one of
the key visual criteria is skin color (Bruce and Langton 1994; Hill, Bruce, and
Akamatsu 1995; Russell and Sinha 2007; Russell et al. 2006; Tarr et al. 2001;
Tarr, Rossion, and Doerschner 2002). In particular, these neurons use two
aspects of facial color: hue (degree of brownness and ruddiness) and luminosity
(degree of contrast between lightness of facial skin versus darkness of lip/eye
area). Hue is the fast channel for gender identification. If the face is too
far away or the lighting too dim, this mental mechanism will switch to the
slower but more accurate channel of luminosity (Dupuis-Roy et al. 2009; Nestor
and Tarr 2008a; Nestor and Tarr 2008b; Tarr et al. 2001; Tarr, Rossion, and
Doerschner 2002).
It
has been shown that an observer can identify the gender of a face even if the
image is blurred and differs only in color (Tarr et al. 2001). Indeed, facial
color seems especially crucial if face shape is not clearly visible (Yip and
Sinha 2002).
Even
when not observing a human face we unconsciously associate darkness with men
and lightness with women. This was shown in a series of experiments with Dutch,
Portuguese, and Turkish participants. In the first one, personal names were
gender-recognized faster when male names were presented in black and female
names in white than when the combinations were reversed. In the second
experiment, very briefly appearing black and white blobs had to be classified
by gender; the former were classified predominantly as male and the latter as
female. Finally, in an eye-tracking experiment, observation was longer and
fixation more frequent when a black or dark object was associated with a male
character and a white or light object with a female character (Semin et al.
2018). Similar results come from a word-association test with Navajo
participants: the color black was perceived as more potent and masculine and
the color white as more active and feminine (Osgood 1960, p. 165).
We
therefore perceive skin color, and especially facial color, through the lens of
a mental mechanism that initially arose for gender recognition. This may
explain not only why lighter skin is subconsciously perceived as feminine but
also why women have sought to accentuate this relative pallor in a wide range
of cultures and time periods, most often by avoiding the sun and wearing
protective clothing (Frost 2010, pp. 120-123). To the same end, and often
independently in different geographic regions, powders have been created from
white clay, lime, chalk, or gypsum for the purpose of lightening women’s facial
color or increasing its contrast with lip/eye color (Russell 2003; Russell
2009; Russell 2010).
This
cult of feminine whiteness reached its height in a zone stretching from the
Mediterranean through the Middle East and into South and East Asia. Here, the
sex difference in skin color could develop to its fullest, without being
constrained by the ceiling of very light pigmentation or the floor of very dark
pigmentation. Here too were invented the first cosmetics for women, including
powders to lighten skin artificially, and accessories to keep skin untanned
(parasols, long gloves, conical or wide-brimmed hats). Finally, here too were
created the first works of prose, poetry, and visual art, often on the theme of
female beauty, including feminine whiteness. The collective imagination thus
became populated with women much fairer than their real-life counterparts.
The advent of the
tanned look and the end of feminine whiteness
This
cult of feminine whiteness came to an end in the Western world when women
embraced the tanned look in the early 20th century. This look began as a side
effect of heliotherapy, i.e., the use of sun baths and sun lamps to treat
rickets, tuberculosis of the skin, and other cutaneous diseases. By 1929 it had
become a fashion, to the surprise of observers like this New York Times
journalist: "Idealists would like to believe that the people,
investigating the medical doctrine and accepting it as sober fact, went
deliberately forth to get what was good for them, and took to sun baths with an
avidity they had never shown for spinach, sleep, or orthopedic shoes"
(Segrave 2005, p. 35). The new fad had become especially popular with women
and, as such, entered into the boyish look of the 1920s: bobbed hair, large
shoulders, small bust, narrow hips, and long legs. The intent was to evoke the
image of a boy on the brink of puberty, as shown by the French name of this
fashion trend: la garçonne.
While
the first nudist movements were organizing, la
garçonne freely exhibited her athletic body and enjoyed the benefits of
heliotherapy. The 1920s made tanning fashionable. Translucent skin and pale
complexions were relegated to the theatre prop room of fin de siècle romanticism. Thus disappeared an element of gender
differentiation. The contrast of flesh colors, brown and copper for the man,
white, pink, and ivory for the woman, had been a constant since Ancient Egypt
in literary and pictorial representations. (Bard 1998, p. 41)
The
tanned look tapped into an erotic response that had previously been
marginalized and stigmatized. In Victorian era novels, the "dark
lady" appears as an "impetuous," "ardent," and
"passionate" object of short-lived romances (Carpenter 1936, p. 254).
Similarly, in French novels of the same era "[t]he love incarnated by
brown women appears as the conceptual equivalent of a devouring femininity,
thus making them similar to the mythical figure of Lilith" (Atzenhoffer
2011, p. 6). This motif goes back at least to the Middle Ages in various
European cultures and points to an alternate mode of eroticism:
[...]
dark girls [...] are inevitably imagined as sexually more available than their
fairer sisters, with whom they are implicitly or explicitly contrasted. In
addition, the change of a girl's complexion, such as being burned by the sun,
is to be understood as symbolic of her having crossed a sexual threshold
without the benefit of marriage. (Vasvari 1999)
European
folklore has sayings along the lines of "the darker the berry, the sweeter
the juice," such as the following from a Venetian folk-poem:
"My
lady mother always told me that I should never be enamoured of white
roses," says a sententious young man; "she told me that I should love
the little mulberries, which are sweeter than honey." "Cara
mora," mora, or mulberry,
meaning brunette, is an ordinary caressing term. (Martinengo-Cesaresco 1886, p.
95)
This
is consistent with the idea that women evolved lighter skin not as a means to
stimulate male sexual arousal but rather as a means to modify such arousal by
reducing aggressiveness in the male observer and inducing feelings of care.
Women are thus lighter-skinned for the same reason that they have less body
hair, a higher pitch of voice, and a more babyish face. These are all key
aspects of Konrad Lorenz's Kindchenschema
(Lorenz 1971, pp. 154-164). Another ethologist, Richard Russell, was the first
to use this concept to explain women's lighter skin:
I
believe the sexual differences in skin color resulted from female whiteness
being selected for because it is opposite the threat coloration, although the
selection pressures may have been rather mild. Light skin seems to be more
paedomorphic, since individuals of all races tend to darken with age. Even in
the gorilla, the most heavily pigmented of the hominoids, the young are born
with very little pigment. [...] Thus, a lighter colored individual may present
a less threatening, more juvenile image. (Guthrie 1970)
This
explanation is supported by a two-part study where each male participant was
first shown pictures of women and asked to rate their attractiveness. Women
with lighter skin were not rated more attractive than those with darker skin.
In the second part, eye movements were tracked, and it was found that women
with lighter skin were viewed for a longer time. The longer duration could
indicate a slower rise and fall in sexual arousal (Garza et al. 2016). Women's
lighter skin may thus lengthen and pacify male sexual arousal via a Kindchenschema effect.
Conclusion
Today,
skin color is seen as a mark of ethnic identity, yet this is not the sole
meaning it has had for humans. For most of history and prehistory it was seen as
a mark of male or female identity.
This
older meaning has received much less interest, even in academia. It is perhaps
no coincidence that scholarly interest has come disproportionately from
non-Westerners. In contrast, Western academics, and Americans in particular,
generally view the psychological meaning of skin color as a legacy of slavery.
References
Atzenhoffer, R. (2011). Les hommes préfèrent les blondes. Les
lectrices aussi. Effet de psychologie, horizons idéologiques et valeurs morales
des héroïnes dans l'œuvre romanesque de H. Courths-Mahler. Colloque
national (CNRIUT), Villeneuve d'Ascq, 8-10 juin 2009,
Bard, C. (1998). Les garçonnes. Modes et fantasmes des Années
folles. Paris:
Flammarion.
Baudouin,
J.-Y., and R. Brochard. (2011). Gender-based prototype formation in face
recognition. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37(4): 888-898.
Bestelmeyer,
P.E.G., B.C. Jones, L.M. DeBruine, A.C. Little, D.I. Perrett, A. Schneider,
L.L.M. Welling, C.A. Conway. (2008). Sex-contingent face aftereffects depend on
perceptual category rather than structural encoding. Cognition 107(1): 353-365.
Bruce,
V., and S. Langton. (1994). The use of pigmentation and shading information in
recognising the sex and identities of faces. Perception 23(7): 803-822.
Carpenter,
F. I. (1936). Puritans preferred blondes. The heroines of Melville and
Hawthorne. New England Quarterly
9(2): 253-272.
Dupuis-Roy,
N., I. Fortin, D. Fiset, and F. Gosselin. (2009). Uncovering gender
discrimination cues in a realistic setting. Journal
of Vision 9(2): 10, 1-8.
Edwards,
E.A., and S.Q. Duntley. (1939). The pigments and color of living human skin. American Journal of Anatomy 65(1): 1-33.
Edwards,
E.A., and S.Q. Duntley. (1949). Cutaneous vascular changes in women in
reference to the menstrual cycle and ovariectomy. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology 57(3): 501-509.
Edwards,
E.A., J.B. Hamilton, S.Q. Duntley, and G. Hubert. (1941). Cutaneous vascular
and pigmentary changes in castrate and eunuchoid men. Endocrinology 28(1): 119-128. https://doi.org/10.1210/endo-28-1-119
Frost, P. (2010). Femmes claires, hommes foncés. Les racines
oubliées du colorisme. Quebec City: Les Presses de l'Université Laval, 202
p.
Garn, S.M., S. Selby, and M.R.
Crawford. (1956).
Skin reflectance studies in children and adults. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 14:101-117.
Garza,
R., R.R. Heredia, and A.B. Cieslicka. (2016). Male and female perception of
physical attractiveness. An eye movement study. Evolutionary Psychology 14(1): 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916631614
.
Guthrie,
R.D. (1970). Evolution of human threat display organs. In T. Dobzhansky, M.K.
Hecht, and W.C. Steere (Eds.) Evolutionary
Biology 4: 257-302. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.
Jaquet,
E., and G. Rhodes. (2008). Face aftereffects indicate dissociable, but not
distinct, coding of male and female faces. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 34(1):
101-112.
Kalla,
A.K. (1973). Ageing and sex differences in human skin pigmentation. Zeitschrift für Morphologie und
Anthropologie 65(1): 29-33.
Hill,
H., V. Bruce, and S. Akamatsu. (1995). Perceiving the sex and race of faces:
The role of shape and colour. Proceedings
of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 261(1362): 367-373.
Little,
A.C., L.M. DeBruine, and B.C. Jones. (2005). Sex-contingent face after-effects
suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,
Series B 272(1578): 2283-2287.
Lorenz,
K. (1971). Studies in Animal and Human
Behaviour, vol. 2. London: Methuen & Co.
Manning,
J.T., P.E. Bundred, and F.M. Mather. (2004). Second to fourth digit ratio,
sexual selection, and skin colour. Evolution
and Human Behavior 25: 38-50.
Martinengo-Cesaresco,
E. (1886). Essays in the Study of
Folk-Songs. London: J.M. Dent & Sons.
Mazess,
R.B. (1967). Skin color in Bahamian Negroes. Human Biology 39:145-154.
Mesa,
M.S. (1983). Analyse de la variabilité de la pigmentation de la
peau durant la croissance. Bulletin et
mémoires de la Société d'Anthropologie de Paris, t. 10 série 13: 49-60.
Nestor,
A., and M.J. Tarr. (2008a). The segmental structure of faces and its use in
gender recognition. Journal of Vision
8(7): 7, 1-12, http://journalofvision.org/8/7/7/, https://doi.org/10.1167/8.7.7.
Nestor,
A., and M.J. Tarr. (2008b). Gender recognition of human faces using color. Psychological Science 19(12): 1242-1246.
Osgood,
C.E. (1960). The cross-cultural generality of visual-verbal synesthetic
tendencies. Behavioral Science 5(2):
146-169.
Russell,
R. (2003). Sex, beauty, and the relative luminance of facial features. Perception 32(9): 1093-1107.
Russell,
R. (2009). A sex difference in facial pigmentation and its exaggeration by
cosmetics. Perception 38(8):
1211-1219.
Russell,
R. (2010). Why cosmetics work. In R. Adams, N. Ambady, K. Nakayama, and S.
Shimojo (Eds.) The science of social
vision (pp. 186-203). New York: Oxford.
Russell,
R. and P. Sinha. (2007). Real-world face recognition: The importance of surface
reflectance properties. Perception
36(9): 1368-1374.
Russell,
R., P. Sinha, I. Biederman, and M. Nederhouser. (2006). Is pigmentation
important for face recognition? Evidence from contrast negation. Perception 35:749-759.
Segrave,
K. (2005). Suntanning in 20th Century
America. Jefferson (North Carolina): McFarland & Company.
Semin,
G.R., T. Palma, C. Acaturk, and A. Dziuba. (2018). Gender is not simply a
matter of black and white, or is it? Philosophical
Transactions of The Royal Society B Biological Sciences 373(1752):20170126
Siiteri,
P.K., and P.C. MacDonald. (1973).Role of extraglandular estrogen in human
endocrinology, in S.R. Geiger (ed.). Handbook
of Physiology, vol. 2, part 1. Washington, D.C.: American Physiology
Society, Section 7, pp. 615-629.
Sitek,
A., S. Koziel, A. Kasielska-Trojan, and B. Antoszewski. (2018). Do skin and
hair pigmentation in prepubertal and early pubertal stages correlate with
2D:4D? American Journal of Human Biology,
early view
Tarr,
M.J., D. Kersten, Y. Cheng, and B. Rossion. (2001). It's Pat! Sexing faces
using only red and green. Journal of
Vision 1(3): 337, 337a.
Tarr,
M. J., B. Rossion, and K. Doerschner. (2002). Men are from Mars, women are from
Venus: Behavioral and neural correlates of face sexing using color. Journal of Vision 2(7): 598, 598a,
Thorstenson,
C.A. (2018). The social psychophysics of human face color: Review and
recommendations. Social Cognition 36(2):
247-273
Vasvari,
L.O. (1999). A comparative approach to European folk poetry and the erotic
wedding motif. CLCWeb. Comparative
Literature and Culture 1(4)
Yip,
A.W., and P. Sinha. (2002). Contribution of color to face recognition. Perception 31(8): 995-1003.