Estonian
women at a song festival (Wikicommons – Anastasia Lakhtikova)
Estonian women had more reproductive success during the late 20th century if they possessed a more masculine body build, narrower hips, and shorter legs. Such women married earlier and were less likely to stay on the mate market as long as possible.
Human
evolution didn’t end in the Pleistocene. In fact, there has been more genetic
change within our species over the past 10,000 years than over the previous
100,000, and perhaps more than over the previous million. The growing
importance of culture did not slow down the pace of genetic change. In fact,
culture became the main driving force of genetic evolution by replacing
adaptation to a limited number of natural environments with adaptation to an ever-widening
range of cultural environments (Cochran and Harpending 2009; Hawks et al. 2007;
Rinaldi 2017).
Two
years ago, I reviewed a study on recent evolution in the Estonian population
(Frost 2020; Hõrak and Valge 2015). Among Estonians born between 1937 and 1962,
women with only primary education had 0.5 to 0.75 more children than did women
with tertiary education. This difference in reproductive success correlated
with difference in cranial volume: children with larger crania were more likely
to go on to secondary or tertiary education, independently of sex,
socioeconomic position, and rural vs urban origin (Valge et al. 2019). Thus,
for Estonian women in the late 20th century, higher education decreased
fertility, probably by postponing the age of marriage.
That
finding was found only for women. Perhaps Estonian men with higher education
enjoyed greater reproductive success, in which case selection for less
intelligent women may have been cancelled out by selection for more intelligent
men.
The
same research team has now published a new study of the same dataset, this time
on both sexes. They confirm the original finding that female fertility
correlated negatively with education and cranial volume. As for male fertility,
although it correlated positively with education, the most fertile males had
only average cranial volume. The authors had no explanation for that finding:
Stabilizing selection on the cranial
volume of boys was an unexpected result, given that cranial volume in our study
population predicts educational attainment independently of sex, socioeconomic
background, and height. Since educational attainment was a strong predictor of
fatherhood in our study, we would have expected positive directional selection
on cranial volume. However, we found only evidence for stabilizing selection
(Valge et al. 2022)
Perhaps
women prefer men who are well-educated but not excessively intelligent. As one
goes farther and farther away from the mean IQ of a population, higher intelligence
becomes more and more often due to genetic “accidents”—unusual genetic variants
or combinations of variants that may adversely affect other aspects of mind and
behavior. A very intelligent person may seem autistic or have poor social
skills.
The
new study also shows that women had greater reproductive success if they
possessed a more masculine body build, narrower hips, and shorter legs. That
finding may seem counterintuitive. Don’t men prefer feminine-looking women? They
do. However, as the authors show by citing earlier findings, shorter women are
also less selective and likelier to marry earlier:
Similar reasoning might also explain why
selection favored girls with masculine body build, narrow hips, and absolutely
and relatively shorter legs in our study. If choosiness in women increases with
desirability, this could lead to women with more feminine phenotypes engaging
in a more time-consuming mate selection process, delaying their age of first
birth, and thereby negatively affecting reproduction. (Valge et al. 2022)
Finally,
the new Estonian study shows that heavier and stronger boys had more
reproductive success.
The results relating to height and
strength are consistent with studies of sexual selection showing that men who
are taller, stronger, and more physically fit are generally perceived as more
physically attractive by women, and therefore, have better opportunities for
partnering and becoming a father. For instance, in a sample of Polish men born
in the 1930s, childless men appeared significantly shorter than those with at
least one child. In West Point graduates, the number of children increased
linearly with height because taller men had higher probabilities of marrying
more than once. Barclay and Kolk showed in a sample of 405,427 Swedish
conscripts born between 1965 and 1972 that men in the lowest deciles of height,
and in particular, physical fitness in early adulthood, had the lowest
probabilities of transition to parenthood. (Valge et al. 2022)
Final thoughts
This
is a study of Estonians who were born more than a half-century ago, long before
the breakup of the Soviet Union. Things may be different now. Estonians have
rapidly converged on Western social, behavioral, and ideological norms over the
past three decades. Although their country is nominally independent, they are
now strongly influenced by the inflow of Western culture via the media, and
this new media environment is having a decisive impact on how they think and
act (Karlin 2018).
Estonia
is generally following the lead of the West. With respect to education and fertility,
the negative correlation has become stronger throughout the West: “In all
countries [Australia, United States, Norway, Sweden], however, education is
negatively associated with childbearing across partnerships, and the
differentials increased from the 1970s to the 2000s” (Thomson et al. 2014).
This
differential is increasing not only between families but also within
“families.” Second and third children are born increasingly to women who have
divorced and are in relationships with low-quality fathers who often seem to be
little more than sperm donors. In Norway, multi-partner fatherhood has become
most common among men with the lowest level of education (10 years of
schooling, "i.e., compulsory education"):
At age 45, about 15 percent of all men in
the 1960-62 cohort with a compulsory education had had children with more than
one woman, compared to about 5 percent among men with a tertiary degree. If
looking at fathers only (Figure 6), the pattern becomes even more pronounced. At
the lowest educational level, 19.3 percent of those who had become fathers, had
children with more than one woman, compared to 6.1 percent of those at the
highest educational level. (Lappegård et al. 2011)
This
trend may partly explain the slowing down and reversal of the Flynn effect,
i.e., the steady rise in mean IQ over the 20th century. There is some debate
over whether the Flynn effect was a real increase in intelligence or simply an
increase in familiarity with doing tests. In any case, its reversal seems real
enough.
With
respect to Norway, Bratsberg and Rogeberg (2018) have shown that the decline in
mean IQ can be explained by “within-family variation.” In other words, mean IQ
is declining among people who supposedly share the same genetic background,
i.e., siblings. In Norway, however, siblings are increasingly half-siblings.
Among Norwegian women with only two children, 13.4% have had them by more than
one man. The figure rises to 24.9% among those with three children, 36.2% among
those with four children, and 41.2% among those with five children (Thomson et
al. 2014).
The
family unit is decomposing throughout the West. It is becoming little more than
an administrative entity that can be repeatedly dissolved and reconstituted (Frost
2018a; Frost 2018b).
References
Bratsberg,
B., and O. Rogeberg. (2018). Flynn effect and its reversal are both
environmentally caused. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences 115 (26) 6674-6678
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1718793115
Cochran,
G. and H. Harpending. (2009). The 10,000
Year Explosion: How Civilization Accelerated Human Evolution. Basic Books:
New York.
Frost, P. (2018a). Why is IQ declining in Norway? Evo and Proud, June 19. https://evoandproud.blogspot.com/2018/06/why-is-iq-declining-in-norway.html
Frost, P. (2018b). Yes, the decline is genetic. Evo and Proud, June 26. https://evoandproud.blogspot.com/2018/06/yes-decline-is-genetic.html
Frost, P. (2020). Declining intelligence in the 20th century: the case of Estonia. Evo and Proud, August 3. https://evoandproud.blogspot.com/2020/08/declining-intelligence-in-20th-century.html
Hawks,
J., E.T. Wang, G.M. Cochran, H.C. Harpending, and R.K. Moyzis. (2007). Recent
acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 104: 20753-20758. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707650104
Hõrak,
P., and M. Valge. (2015). Why did children grow so well at hard times? The
ultimate importance of pathogen control during puberty. Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health
(1): 167–178, https://doi.org/10.1093/emph/eov017
Karlin, A. (2018). Gay marriage in Estonia. The Unz Review, October 30. https://unz.com/akarlin/estonian-freezer/
Lappegård, T., Rønsen, M., and Skrede, K. (2011). Fatherhood and fertility. Fathering 9: 103-120. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.839.2752&rep=rep1&type=pdf
Rinaldi,
A. (2017). We're on a road to nowhere. Culture and adaptation to the
environment are driving human evolution, but the destination of this journey is
unpredictable. EMBO reports 18:
2094-2100. https://doi.org/10.15252/embr.201745399
Thomson, E., T. Lappegård, M. Carlson, A. Evans, and E. Gray (2014). Childbearing across partnerships in Australia, the United States, Norway, and Sweden. Demography 51(2): 485-508. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-013-0273-6
Valge,
M., R. Meitern, and P. Hõrak. (2022).
Sexually antagonistic selection on educational attainment and body size in
Estonian children. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences Early view https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14859