Originally
from south China, Austronesians spread successively outward to Taiwan,
Southeast Asia, and Oceania. Was farming the secret of their success? Or was it
their mental makeup? (source: French Wikipedia - Maulucioni)
About
10,000 years ago, the pace of human genetic evolution rose a hundred-fold
(Hawks et al., 2007). Our ancestors were no longer adapting to slowly changing
physical environments. They were adapting to rapidly evolving cultural
environments.
What,
exactly, caused this speed-up? The usual answer is the shift from hunting and
gathering to farming, which in turn caused other changes. People were becoming
sedentary and living in ever larger communities: villages, towns, and finally
cities. Farming also produced a food surplus to be stored for future use,
thereby providing powerful men with the means to bankroll a growing number of
servants, soldiers, and other hangers-on. Thus began the formation of early
states. And thus ended the primitive equality of hunter-gatherers.
But
is that the whole story? Was farming the trigger for this chain of events? Or
did something earlier get things going? More and more anthropologists are taking
a closer look at what happened just before the advent of farming, a period
called the "Broad Spectrum Revolution":
All
Paleolithic hominids lived by hunting and collecting wild foods, an aspect of
existence that began to disappear only with the emergence of the farming and
herding societies of the Neolithic ≤10,000 years ago (10 KYA). What are the
roots of this remarkable economic transformation? The answer lies in equally
revolutionary changes that took place within certain stone age cultures several
millennia before. In 1968, Lewis R. Binford noted what appeared to be
substantial diversification of human diets in middle- and high-latitude Europe
at the end of the Paleolithic, roughly 12-8 KYA. Rapid diversification in
hunting, food processing, and food storage equipment generally accompanied
dietary shifts, symptoms of intensified use of habitats, and fuller
exploitation of the potential foodstuffs they contained. (Stiner, 2001).
Farming
thus came on the heels of a broader cultural, behavioral, and even psychological
revolution. It is this broader change, rather than farming alone, that probably
caused many supposedly farming-related events, such as the rapid spread of
certain agricultural peoples into territories that formerly belonged to hunter-gatherers.
Examples include the Bantu expansion, which began about 4,000 years ago along
the Nigerian/Cameroun border and spread east and south, eventually throughout
almost all of central, eastern and southern Africa. There was also the more
controversial Neolithic expansion, which started about 10,000 years ago in
present-day Turkey and pushed north and west into Europe. Finally, there was the
Austronesian expansion, which began over 6,000 years ago when Malay-speaking
farmers crossed from south China to Taiwan. Then, some 4,000 years ago, they
began to push rapidly into Southeast Asia and Oceania, finally reaching places
as far apart as Easter Island and Madagascar.
Farming
is said to have fueled all three demographic expansions. It created more food,
which in turn created more people, who then bulldozed the less numerous
hunter-gatherers out of existence. Yet this theoretical model does not fit the
Austronesian expansion very well, as anthropologist Roger Blench points out:
The
[existing] model proposes that it was the Austronesian adoption of an
agricultural package, including rice, pigs and chickens, which allowed them to
colonise Island SE Asia at the expense of resident hunter-gatherers. However,
archaeology has signally failed to confirm this model. Early sites show very
similar dates across a wide geographical area, suggesting that the first phase
of post-Taiwan Austronesian expansion took place extremely rapidly. Pigs, dogs
and chickens have been shown to arrive in ISEA via other routes and rice is
conspicuously absent in most places. This paper argues that this model has
effectively inverted the actual situation, and that the Austronesian expansion
was the consequence of a failed agricultural revolution and a reversion to
opportunistic foraging. (Blench, 2014)
There
was no "package" of domesticated plants and animals that enabled
Austronesians to overwhelm the earlier inhabitants of Southeast Asia:
No
remains of cereals of the relevant antiquity have been found in the Northern
Philippines. Even today, the characteristic millets of the Asian mainland are
barely represented in ISEA agriculture. Of the dogs, pigs and chickens
originally thought to be part of the Austronesian 'package' only pigs cross the
Taiwan Strait, and these now seem to be a local domestication not ancestral to
the domestic pigs which are generally part of the Austronesian world. The
apparent reconstructions for names of these domestic species formed part of the
argument for their salience, but as Blench (2012) points out, these were based
on chains of loanwords giving an appearance of spurious antiquity. (Blench, 2014)
Not
only did the Austronesians bring a limited farming package to Southeast Asia, the
existing peoples there already practiced a mix of farming and foraging:
Ellen
(1988) describes this type of mixed vegeculture and arboriculture, a sedentary
lifestyle based around sago extraction, for Seram in Eastern Indonesia. Stark
(1996) touches on this hypothesis in a discussion of the archaeology of Eastern
Indonesia, and Kyle Latinis (2000) discusses the broader role of arboriculture
in early subsistence in ISEA. Hunt & Rushworth (2005) report evidence for
disturbance in the tropical lowland forest at Niah, Sarawak, Malaysian Borneo
at 6000 BP which they attribute to cultivation. Huw Barton (2012) has evidence
from starch on stone pounders in the Kelabit highlands for palm granules
earlier than 6500 BP. (Blench, 2014)
The
incoming Austronesians really brought little, agriculturally speaking. In some
cases, they actually reverted to foraging. In other cases, they adopted various
crops and farm animals from the locals.
The
picture is diverse, suggesting continued foraging, vegeculture, and sago starch
extraction. Hence the concept that the maritime Austronesians reverted to a
type of subsistence based on fishing, trading, possibly raiding and exchange of
prestige goods. With their advanced sailing technology, they were well-placed
to carry high-value goods from one exchange site to another. Their encounter
with the Melanesian populations in Eastern Indonesia is certainly responsible
for their adoption of vegeculture and domestic animals, but they were willing
to drop and re-adopt these according to the circumstances of individual
cultures they encountered. (Blench, 2014)
The
Austronesians are often treated in the existing literature as a type-society
for demographic expansion, with agriculture the underlying engine of growth.
This is in increasing disaccord with the archaeology of the region, and this
paper will suggest that the explanation is almost its inverse, that they
succeeded precisely because they strategically reverted to foraging. (Blench, 2014)
What
advantage, then, did the Austronesians have over the natives they displaced?
There must have been something to offset the "home team" advantage of
the natives, who knew the local environment better than anyone else.
The
Austronesian advantage seems to have been threefold: (1) a more flexible and
innovative approach; (2) a less present-oriented time orientation that extended
further into the past and the future; and (3) a less individualistic approach
to life that made collective goods and goals more possible.
Flexibility and innovation
Blench
(2014) states: “Austronesians quickly reinvented themselves, incorporating
regional innovations into their cultural repertoire. Apart from their own
distinctive pottery, they must have quickly seized on other early trade
possibilities, obsidian, stone axes, woven goods and baskets. By the time they
begin to reach uninhabited islands they have constructed Austronesian culture
out of fragmentary elements adopted from a wide range of sources.”
Time orientation
The
Austronesians saw themselves as the embodiment of lineages that stretched
backwards into the past and forwards into the future. Preservation of
genealogies seems to have been a typical cultural trait of Austronesian
peoples, and this ancestor worship fostered a widespread desire among them to
become revered ancestors. Quoting an earlier author, Blench (2014) points to
"a culturally sanctioned desire to found new settlements in order to
become a revered or even deified founder ancestor in the genealogies of future
generations." The rapidity of the Austronesian expansion was thus, in
part, ideologically driven.
Collectivism
Blench
(2014) refers to "the striking differences between Austronesians in
contact with Melanesians, whose typical social structures were individualist,
acephalous and marked by great diversity in approaches to religion and
belief." Austronesian society was more hierarchical with caste-like
elements, particularly priestly castes who acted as repositories of esoteric
knowledge. Belief-systems were more homogeneous. Austronesian peoples have long
shown evidence of strong religiosity, even those who have not converted
to Hinduism, Islam, or Catholicism.
Conclusion
One
of my professors, Bernard Arcand, would talk to us about the hunter-gatherers
of Upper Amazonia and their indifference to farming. They saw it as something
akin to slavery and couldn't understand why anyone would want to stay put in
one place and toil in the fields all day. Attempts to teach them the benefits
of farming typically failed. Benefits? What benefits?
There
has to be a change in mental makeup before farming becomes possible. People must
become willing to exchange short-term pain for long-term gain. They must accept
monotony and sedentary living. They must live in larger communities with people
who are not necessarily close kin. And they must get used to bland,
nutrient-poor food.
Anthropology
has long tried to explain human societies in terms of their modes of
subsistence: hunting and gathering, farming, industrial capitalism. This
material basis of society is thus the source of our mental makeup. In reality, the
two have coevolved with each other. Moreover, when humans adapt to a certain
mode of subsistence, they may become "pre-adapted" to later ones. We
associate the dawn of civilization with a shift toward future time orientation
and a resulting complexification of technology, yet this shift seems to have
first begun among hunter-gatherers of the sub-Arctic, where the yearly cycle
required development of technologies for storage, meat refrigeration, and heat
conservation, as well as other means to collect unpredictable and widely
dispersed resources. This 'first industrial revolution' pre-adapted early
modern humans for later cultural developments in places farther south.
It is
perhaps no coincidence that most of the human gene pool ultimately came from
people who, more than 10,000 years ago, roamed the northern wastes of Eurasia.
Such people were ancestral to many southward-pushing demographic expansions,
including the one that would give rise to the Austronesians (Frost, 2014).
References
Blench,
R. (2014). The Austronesians: An agricultural revolution that failed, To be
presented at the Second International
Conference on Taiwan Indigenous Peoples, 15-17 September 2014 Shung Ye Museum, Taipei,
Taiwan
http://rb.rowbory.co.uk/Archaeology/Oceania/Blench%20Austronesian%20Taipei%202014%20agric%20failed.pdf
Cochran,
G. and H. Harpending. (2009). The 10,000
Year Explosion: How Civilizations Accelerated Human Evolution, Basic Books,
New York.
Frost,
P. (2014). The first industrial revolution, Evo
and Proud, January 18
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2014/01/the-first-industrial-revolution.html
Hawks,
J., E.T. Wang, G.M. Cochran, H.C. Harpending, & R.K. Moyzis. (2007). Recent
acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 104, 20753-20758.
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2410101/
Stiner,
M.C. (2001). Thirty years on the "Broad Spectrum Revolution" and
paleolithic demography, Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, 98
(13), 6993-6996.
http://hatayup.web.arizona.edu/pubs/stiner2001.pdf
10 comments:
Two questions:
First, what does the genetic data say about admixture between Austronesian and Papuan/Melanesian peoples over the range of Austronesian dispersal? How much do, say, Tahitians differ from, say, Filipinos or Javanese in this regard?
Second, what's the score on average IQ in Austronesian versus Melanesian populations? I seem to recall from Piffer's study earlier this year that Melanesian groups had an average of 25 percent incidence of the gene variants he surveyed - lower than Eurasians but higher than sub-Saharan African. But I don't remember what the Indonesian/Micronesian/Polynesian figures were, if indeed they were included.
If longer future-orientation (=higher intelligence) is a crusial part of becoming farmers, then what was the selection pressure in the case of Bantu-people?
Not climate for sure.
This pattern of rapid dispersals
followed by pauses strongly resembles the ‘punctuated equilibrium’ model of evolution delineated by Niles Eldredge and Stephen J. Gould (1972).
Heh. Not sure they would appreciate the reference.
Very fine piece, Peter. It sounds to me like these Austronesians developed some very flexible, opportunity seeking minds. Could we call that quality something like "g"?
Peter - I had thought the expansion into Oceania began about 3,000 years ago I recall reading that archaeological evidence in Guam indicates human presence there since about 1000 BC and this is among the earliest known occupied sites. Is there new evidence for earlier presence of humans in Oceania?
By the term "Oceania" I am excluding New Guinea and nearby islands such as the Solomon's etc. where archaeological evidence of human presence goes back at least 30,000 years.
Peter - Thanks for the references you provided. I lived in Guam when I was a child and I have a special interest in Oceania.
Adaptationist arguments that require a spatially local fitness advantage are not necessary to explain the displacement of local Micronesian populations by the Austronesians.
All it requires is an already well-established migration pressure. With superior ships, the incoming group would have been able to relatively quickly populate any sites that experienced temporary demographic setbacks - perturbations, disturbances. Famine, war, disease, whatever it was, if the new group could find and utilize the site,... well, there was no competition on the relevant time scale at the smaller spatial scale.
BenK - What is the evidence for populations in the Micronesians who did not speak Austronesian languages? I believe that the archaeological evidence in Guam dating from 1000 BC. indicates probable migration from the Phillipines and continuity since then. Chamorro is an Austronesian language.
The funny thing about the archaeological evidence from the Micronesians is that except for the Marianas the earliest dated sites are in the East and some of the youngest in Yap and Palau. So it looks like a migration from East to West but this may be an artifact of just which sites have been discovered so far.
Ben,
That's the traditional model, i.e., a steady demographic expansion driven by more numerous farmers at the expense of less numerous hunter-gatherers.
Blench argues, convincingly in my view, that this model is inadequate. The Austronesian expansion involved very rapid dispersals, often within a time range of a few hundred years.
Peter, Jim;
Thank you, I'm not an expert at this at all. I'm just suggesting that there needn't be any advantage besides the ability to move/migrate - i.e. the boats - if populations fluctuate rather severely for one or both types of people. In fact, local fitnesses could be the inverse.
Post a Comment