Navaho woman with a child on cradleboard.
see video on cross-cultural differences in newborn behavior, Daniel Freedman, 1974 (posted by hbd chick)
In
my last post I discussed recent research on mental differences between
Europeans and Chinese people. The latter are less prone to boredom. They think
less abstractly and more relationally. They're less individualistic, and less
likely to punish friends for dishonesty. Mental
differences also seem to exist within China, depending on whether one comes from a region that
historically grew rice or one that historically grew wheat. Chinese from
wheat-growing regions are closer to Europeans in mentality.
Are
these differences inborn? Or are they due to upbringing? The second explanation
is hard to reconcile with the fact that the regional differences within China
involved urban residents who had never lived on a farm of any sort.
Almost
a half-century ago, these questions interested the American psychologist Daniel
Freedman and his wife Nina Chinn Freedman. They examined 24 Chinese-American
and 24 Euro-American newborns whose parents were otherwise similar in age,
economic class, and number of previous children. The two groups nonetheless
behaved differently. The Euro-American babies cried more easily, were harder to
console, and would immediately turn their faces aside if placed face down on a
sheet. In contrast, the Chinese-American babies accepted almost any position
without crying or resisting. When a light was shone in their eyes, the
Euro-American babies would continue to blink long after the Chinese-American
babies had stopped blinking (Freedman and Freedman, 1969; Freedman, 2004).
These
findings were partially replicated by another American psychologist, Jerome
Kagan, who found that Chinese 4-month-olds cried, fretted, and vocalized less
than Euro-American infants. At older ages, however, the pattern reversed with
Chinese Americans fretting and crying more when separated from their mothers
(Kagan et al., 1978; Kagan et al., 1994).
Is
this response specific to Chinese? Or does it apply to East Asians in general?
In a study of Euro-American, Japanese, and Chinese 11-month olds, the last group
was the least expressive one, being least likely to smile or cry. The Japanese
babies either fell between the two other groups or were like the Euro-American
babies (Camras et al., 1998). When another study looked at Japanese and British
newborns, the latter actually showed more self-quieting activity (Eishima,
1992).
On
the other hand, Navaho babies are even calmer and more adaptable than Chinese
babies (Freedman, 2004). Some anthropologists have attributed this finding to a
traditional practice of tying the baby to a cradleboard. As Freedman pointed
out, however, this practice is now only sporadic among the Navaho.
Freedman
attributed his Chinese and Navaho findings to a general Mongoloid temperament.
If that were the case, infants should behave similarly in other North American
native peoples. A study of Alaskan Inupiaq found young children to be shy but
otherwise no different from Euro-American children. These subjects were,
however, older than Freedman’s, being 3 to 6 years of age (Sprott, 2002).
It
may be that the Navaho differ from other North American native peoples in this
respect. Perhaps, in the past, mortality was higher among those babies who
resisted the cradleboard; over time, they and their temperament would have been
steadily removed from the gene pool. As Freedman noted, "most Navaho
infants calmly accept the board; in fact, many begin to demand it by showing
signs of unrest when off." When Euro-American mothers tried using the
cradleboard, "their babies complained so persistently that they were off
the board in a matter of weeks" (Freedman, 2004).
Infant
calmness can thus arise in relatively simple societies, and not just in
advanced ones as I had argued in my last post. In the Navaho case, there may
have been some kind of parental selection, i.e., through their child-rearing
practices, parents influence what sort of children survive and what sort don't.
In other simple societies, such as among the Australian Aborigines, infant
behavior is much less calm and compliant (Freedman, 2004).
Behavior
can likewise differ between infants from different complex societies. We've
seen this with Chinese-American and Euro-American babies, the latter having a
less easy temperament. A difficult temperament (colic, excessive crying) is
also much more common in babies of Greek or Middle Eastern origin than in
babies of Northwest European or Asian Indian origin (Prior et al., 1987).
In
the future, it would be interesting to find out whether infants differ in
temperament within China, such as between rice-growing and wheat-growing
regions.
But will there be
more research?
There seems to be less and less interest
in this area of research, particularly within the United
States. I can point to several reasons:
-
The behavioral differences between Chinese and Japanese babies must have arisen
over a relatively short span of evolutionary time. Many researchers, even those
who are receptive to HBD thinking, have trouble accepting fast behavioral
evolution, especially below the level of large continental races.
-
American researchers are increasingly interested in the possibility that early
parental interaction, such as reading to children, can stimulate brain
development. Although it is doubtful that parental interaction can explain
differences in newborn behavior, this assumption seems to make people
dismissive of Freedman's work.
-
Since the 1970s, and throughout the Western world, academia has become more
hostile to the possibility of genetic influences on human behavior. This trend
is self-reinforcing, since hiring decisions are biased toward candidates who
believe in environmental determinism.
The
last two points apply much less to East Asian scholars ... or American
ones who are willing to do some of their work offshore.
Right
now, we need to identify the genetic causation for these differences in infant
behavior. One cause may be the 7R allele of the D4 dopamine receptor gene,
which is associated with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and is
very rare in East Asians (Leung et al., 2005). Nonetheless, as with differences
in intellectual capacity, we're probably looking at an accumulation of small
effects at many different genes. Natural selection acts on what genes produce,
and not directly on genes, so there is no reason to believe that a single behavioral
outcome has a single genetic cause. That would be too convenient.
References
Camras,
L.A., H. Oster, J. Campos, R. Campos, T. Ujiie, K. Miyake, L. Wang, and Z.
Meng. (1998). Production of emotional facial expressions in European American,
Japanese, and Chinese infants, Developmental
Psychology, 34, 616-628.
http://www-psych.stanford.edu/~carl/isl/PDFPublications/Production%20of%20emotional%20facial%20expressions%20in%20European%20American,%20Japanese,%20and%20Chinese%20infants..pdf
Eishima,
K. (1992). A study on neonatal behaviour comparing between two groups from
different cultural backgrounds, Early
Human Development, 28, 265-277.
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/037837829290172D
Freedman,
D.G. (2004). Ethnic differences in babies, in L. Dundes (ed.). The Manner Born: Birth Rites in
Cross-Cultural Perspective, pp. 221-232, AltaMira Press.
http://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=ZSizAQAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA221&dq=freedman+chinese+american+newborns&ots=qmXzqhaNE3&sig=eIEw-KCGcciDEDpIiqgdxX9PxJk#v=onepage&q&f=false
Freedman,
D.G., and N.C. Freedman. (1969). Behavioural differences between
Chinese-American and European-American newborns, Nature, 224, 1227.
Kagan,
J., D. Arcus, N. Snidman, W. Feng, J. Hendler, and S. Greene. (1994). Reactivity
in infants: A cross-national comparison, Developmental
Psychology, 30, 342-345.
http://pzacad.pitzer.edu/~dmoore/1994_Kagan%20et%20al_Reactivity%20in%20infants_DP.pdf
Kagan,
J., R. Kearsley, and P. Zelazo. (1978). Infancy:
Its place in human development, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Leung,
P.W.L., C.C. Lee, S.F. Hung, T.P. Ho, C.P. Tang, et al. (2005). Dopamine
receptor D4 (DRD4) gene in Han Chinese children with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Increased prevalence of the
2-repeat allele, American Journal of
Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 133B, 54-56.
http://webcontent.hkcss.org.hk/rh/rpp/HKPaediatricSociety20050630DRD4ADHDChinese.pdf
Prior,
M., E. Garino, A. Sanson, and F. Oberklaid. (1987). Ethnic influences on
"difficult" temperament and behavioural problems in infants, Australian Journal of Psychology, 39, 163-171.
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00049538708259045#.VIMoKuktDcs
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00049538708259045#.VIMoKuktDcs
Sprott,
J.E. (2002). Raising Young Children in an
Alaskan Iñupiaq Village: The Family, Cultural, and Village Environment of
Rearing, Greenwood Publishing Group.
http://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=FjBxXAFqD3QC&oi=fnd&pg=PR13&dq=%22alaskan+inupiaq+newborns+temperament&ots=JfwfOrluQq&sig=B3RSdAUzYqcBPIJyJMS65LGy-Zo#v=onepage&q=temperament&f=false
8 comments:
actually, i didn't post that video. someone else did, and they thought it would be amusing/good advertising for my blog to put my name on it. obviously they've got a quirky sense of humor. (~_^)
The obvious selection for babies that cried easily would be when food was short and the babies crying more got fed more than quiet babies. So a reliable source of food would make for less selection.
I suppose some hunter gatherers are more prone to famine than others just as some agriculturists are. The Navajo herded and did some agriculture so they may have been some food to go around: a favourable environment for a relaxed baby to get fed. And it can't be ignored that mothers can get very annoyed with a constantly crying baby and impose direct selection.
Assuming they were originally a northern hunter gatherer people with seasonal famines, east Asians must have had less docility in the distant past.
I was thinking obvious example of farmer pacification until
"On the other hand, Navaho babies are even calmer and more adaptable than Chinese babies"
hmm.
" Behavior can likewise differ between infants from different complex societies. We've seen this with Chinese-American and Euro-American babies, the latter having a less easy temperament. A difficult temperament (colic, excessive crying) is also much more common in babies of Greek or Middle Eastern origin than in babies of Northwest European or Asian Indian origin (Prior et al., 1987)."
->http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00049538708259045#.VIMoKuktDcs
"Infants of parents born in Greece, Middle Eastern Countries, North America and some countries of Asia were more likely to be rated as showing behaviours characteristic of a difficult temperament...Infants of parents from North Western Europe and India however did not show these difficulties"
??
Who were the "Infants of parents born in...North America and countries of Asia" that were difficult?
Don't forget Chinese moms have been known to suck on boys penises to get them to stop crying.
Here are some references to this cultural item.
http://www.ipt-forensics.com/journal/volume3/j3_4_4.htm
http://slog.thestranger.com/slog/archives/2011/02/17/a-mother-loving-pecks-her-boys-penis
http://message.snopes.com/showthread.php?t=82113
http://www.answers.com/Q/Why_do_mother_suck_on_baby_penis
You clearly have little experience with babies or parenting. Or Chinese babies.
It has crossed many people's minds the Athabaskans might have arrived late: late enough to have indirect Buddhist influences at the time they had arrived. What are Siberian children like?
Post a Comment