Ted
Bundy, 1978, State Archives of Florida (Wikicommons). Outwardly charming but
zero concern for others.
Is
sociopathy an illness? We often think so ... to the point that the word
"sick" has taken on a strange secondary meaning. If we call a
ruthless, self-seeking person "sick," we mean he should be shunned at
all costs. We don't mean he should take an aspirin and get some rest.
Sociopathy
doesn't look like a mental illness, being much less incapacitating than
schizophrenia and most mental disorders. A sociopath can deal with other people
well enough, perhaps too well. As Harpending and Sobus (2015) point out:
It
is a psychopathology because of what sociopaths do to us, and it has
significant legal, political, and moral consequences for all of us. Most
criminals are probably sociopaths according to some definition (the figure of
80% is often quoted).
Sociopaths
regularly present the following characteristics:
-
onset before age 15, childhood hyperactivity, truancy, delinquency, disruption
in school
-
early and often aggressive sexual activity, marital histories of desertion,
non-support, abandonment - persistent lying, cheating, irresponsibility without visible shame
- sudden changes of plan, impulsiveness, unpredictability
- charm and a façade of sensitivity
- high mobility, vagrancy, use of aliases
Sociopaths
follow a life strategy that is adaptive for themselves but ruinous for society.
Harpending and Sobus (2015) argue that they succeed so well because they know
how to manipulate social relationships to their advantage.
Sociopathy
is at least moderately heritable (Hicks et al., 2004). Interestingly, it seems
to cluster with hysteria in first-degree relatives, with sociopathy being
expressed in the males and hysteria in the females. Harpending and Sobus
(2015) argue that "hysteria is the expression in females of the same
genetic material that leads to sociopathy in males." In short,
"sociopathy in females is the result of a greater dose of the genetic
material that leads, in smaller doses, to hysteria, namely, hysteria is mild
sociopathy."
If
sociopathy is adaptive, why does it affect only a minority of us? It seems that
the rest of us have developed counter-strategies of looking for signs of
sociopathy and expelling suspects from society ... and the gene pool. This
is probably why sociopaths tend to be always on the move—if they stay too long
with the same people, they risk being detected and dealt with.
Gene-culture
coevolution
We
adapt to our cultural environment as we do to our natural environment. More so
in fact. The last 10,000 years have seen far more genetic change in our
ancestors than the previous 100,000, this speeding up of evolution being
driven by the entry of humans into an increasingly diverse range of cultural
environments.
Sociopathy
may thus propagate itself more easily in some cultures than in others, with the
result that its incidence may likewise differ from one to another. In a small
band of hunter-gatherers, a sociopath will not last long because he is always
interacting with the same small group of people:
In
a 1976 study anthropologist Jane M. Murphy, then at Harvard University found
that an isolated group of Yupik-speaking Inuits near the Bering Strait had a
term (kunlangeta) they used to
describe "a man who ... repeatedly lies and cheats and steals things and
... takes sexual advantage of many women—someone who does not pay attention to
reprimands and who is always being brought to the elders for punishment."
When Murphy asked an Inuit what the group would typically do with a kunlangeta, he replied, "Somebody
would have pushed him off the ice when nobody else was looking."
(Lilienfeld and Arkowitz, 2007)
In
a larger community, a sociopath may evade detection long enough to reproduce
successfully and pass on his mental traits. Finally, in some cultures he can
use his manipulative skills to dominate the community, becoming a "big
man" and enjoying very good opportunities for reproduction.
This
Pandora's Box was opened when humans gave up hunting and gathering and became
farmers. First, farming supported a much larger population, so it became easier
for sociopaths to move about from one group of unsuspecting people to another.
Second, farming created a food surplus that powerful individuals could use to
support underlings of various sorts: servants, soldiers, scribes, etc. There
was thus a growing class of people who did not directly support themselves and
whose existence depended on their ability to manipulate others.
Finally,
in the tropical zone, farming greatly increased female reproductive autonomy.
Through year-round farming, women could provide for themselves and their
children with less male assistance. Men accordingly shifted their reproductive
strategy from monogamy to polygyny, i.e., from providing for a wife and
children to inseminating as many women as possible. This kind of cultural
environment selected for male seducers and manipulators rather than male
providers. Conversely, it selected for women who feel only an intermittent need
for male companionship and who from time to time are able to coax assistance from
people who are not so inclined:
Ethnographic
descriptions of women who live in social contexts of low male parental
investment portray women who are very demanding. Young women demand help from
kin on behalf of children. When the help is not forthcoming the mothers often
summarily dump or deposit the child or children at the door of a relative who
(in their judgment) will not turn the children away. Women demand gifts from
boyfriends for themselves. (Harpending and Draper, 1988)
In
women, this selection pressure favors a condition known medically as Briquet's
syndrome and more commonly as "hysteria":
When
males are not good risks for parental investment, females will adjust their
behavior accordingly. A common clinical characterization of Briquet's syndrome
is a woman who exaggerates need, who demands high levels of attention and
investment, who deceives herself and others as to her requirements. The
strategy (learned or inherited) makes sense for a woman with high exposure to low
investment males. These males, however, are so fickle and so mobile that they
can be dunned only in the short run. (Harpending and Draper, 1988)
Sociopathic
behavior, be it hysteria or full-blown sociopathy, is not favored in
hunter-gatherers, since both sexes invest heavily in their offspring
and in each other. The selection is for men and women who can bond strongly
with one partner:
[...]
abandonment of the pair bond by either partner is likely to be deadly for the
offspring. Draper (manuscript) finds that men with more children spend more
time hunting than men with fewer dependents; that is to say that more offspring
are directly translated into more parental work for the male. Pennington and
Harpending (manuscript) found that infant mortality among women who had more
than one mate during their reproductive careers was nearly twice as great as
infant mortality of women who had only one husband. [...] In societies of this
type the contexts for the anti-social trait are unfavorable. There will be no
pay-offs for anti-social behavior and the bearer of the trait will be readily
detected and ostracized. (Harpending and Draper, 1988)
Strategy and
counter-strategy
Sociopathy
is therefore not an illness but a strategy. It has been least successful in small
societies where both sexes invest heavily in care for their partners and
offspring. It has been more successful in larger societies, particularly those
where men invest less in partners and offspring. Indeed, because sociopathy
does so well in such contexts, it may have hindered the development of larger
and more complex societies.
In
most large societies, people seek out and expel sociopaths from their local kin
group and treat everyone else with suspicion. The result is the
"amoral familialism" we see throughout much of the world. People prefer to
deal with relatives, hire only relatives for their businesses and, as a rule, act morally only towards relatives. Thus, the
high-trust environment of the family cannot extend to society in general. Among
other things, this is why the market economy has failed to develop
spontaneously over most of the world and over most of history. Without
strong-armed government intervention (military pacification, police, courts,
etc.), markets remain marketplaces—places of exchange that are highly localized
in space and time. The market principle cannot spread to most economic
transactions.
Some
humans have resolved this problem by freeing themselves from the straitjacket
of kinship, by adhering to social rules that apply to everyone, and by
ruthlessly expelling rule breakers wherever they may be. This is the adaptation
that Europeans have developed to the north and west of the Hajnal line. The
relative weakness of kinship ties and, correspondingly, the relative strength
of individualism favored a complex of psychological traits that may be
summarized as follows:
-
capacity to internalize punishment for disobedience of social rules (guilt
proneness).
-
capacity to simulate and then transfer to oneself the emotional states of other
people, especially when such people are affected by rule-breaking either by
oneself or by others (affective empathy).
-
tendency to frame moral rules in universal, absolute terms, i.e., moral
universalism and moral absolutism, as opposed to situational morality based on
kinship. Rule-breakers are likewise condemned in absolute terms and may be
expelled from the entire community, as opposed to being ostracized by close
kin.
The
above mental package brought Northwest Europeans closer than other humans to
the threshold where one could escape the limitations of kinship and organize society
along other lines, notably the market economy, the modern State, and political
ideology. It thus became possible to meet the challenge of creating larger
societies while ensuring compliance with social rules and a high degree of
personal autonomy.
References
Cooke,
D.J. (2003). Cross-Cultural Aspects of Psychopathy, in T. Millon, E. Simonsen,
M. Birket-Smith, and R.D. Davis (eds). Psychopathy:
Antisocial, Criminal, and Violent Behavior, pp. 260-276, Guilford Press.
https://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=LSiBsdxcGigC&oi=fnd&pg=PA260&dq=psychopathy+cross-cultural&ots=nnV3xh4mZZ&sig=FBW4rme_w0tj2REoHQJpqXXSB6Q#v=onepage&q=psychopathy%20cross-cultural&f=false
Harpending,
H. and P. Draper. (1988). Antisocial behavior and the other side of cultural
evolution, in T. E. Moffitt and S.A. Mednick (eds). Biological contributions to crime causation, pp. 110-125, Boston:
Nijhoff.
https://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=Vw7tCAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA293&ots=44dHOZn08j&sig=-2vXa0210OA82u-9Y-HzeEWM-y4#v=onepage&q&f=false
Harpending,
H. and J. Sobus. (2015). Sociopathy as an adaptation, Ethology and Sociobiology, 8,
63-72
https://www.academia.edu/11700522/Sociopathy_as_an_adaptation
Hicks,
B.M., R.F. Krueger, W.G. Iacono, M. McGue, C.J. Patrick. (2004). Family
transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders. A twin-family study, Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 922-928.
http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?articleid=482057
Lilienfeld,
S.O. and H. Arkowitz. (2007). What "Psychopath" Means, Scientific American, 18, 90-91.
http://faculty.fortlewis.edu/burke_b/Abnormal/Abnormal%20Readings/Psychopathy.pdf
7 comments:
Outstanding work!
Have any studies been done on whether rates of antisocial personality disorder (sociopathy) and histrionic personality disorder (hysteria) differ among racial groups? I know that people of African descent have higher rates of Briquet's Syndrome than other groups, which suggests they have higher rates of hysteria.
Yep, Northwest Europeans are the chosen people. All hail the Nords!
Seriously though, I'm sure there are plenty of societies that encourage individualism and have low kinship ties. Also, I don't see a problem with high kinship ties per se, as long as people have a strict moral code, such as in East Asian societies. Confucianism does not mean that close kin are favoured or whatever. It's as morally absolutist as the moral code of the Nords.
In fact, most cultures have pretty much a similar moral code. If I'm reading you correctly, you seem to imply that Nords would only consider their kids as close kin and wouldn't care as much for their fifth cousin's aunt, as say East Asians. But heck, Confucianism emphasises merit, and not cronyism. Sure, you've a duty to your great aunts cousin, but your family doesn't act like the Mafia. East Asian cultures have the best of both, a great moral code and respect for kin. (No, I'm not East Asian)
Cronyism would be a problem in tribal societies like in the middle East.
The most recent genetic research on heritability suggests that psychopathy has a higher level then 'mild'. Here's the most recent and informative I found:
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3387315/
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24796343
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4438762/
Thanks for the post; the anthropological insight adds a nice edge, though I think you also need to include the primate evolutionary aspect, where polygamy might have been more the rule, or at least provide a counter-argument to that possibility: matriarchal societies, for example.
Anonymous,
You know nothing about Confucianism.
"The duke of She informed Confucius, saying, 'Among us here there are those who may be styled upright in conduct. If their father have stolen a sheep, they will bear witness to the fact.' Confucius said, 'Among us, in our part of the country, those who are upright are different from this. The father conceals the misconduct of his son and the son conceals the misconduct of the father. Uprightness is to be found in this.'" (Analects, bk. xiii., c. xviii.)
If this is not kinship-based morality then nothing is.
- Have any studies been done on whether rates of antisocial personality disorder differ among racial groups?
Psychopathy (which is usually a synonym for sociopathy) seems to be more normal in African Americans than in Euro Americans:
"In summary, our findings indicate that low-anxious African-American psychopaths do not demonstrate the emotion paradox or information-processing deficit found in Caucasian psychopaths performing the same lexical decision task."
http://psych.wisc.edu/newman/SecurePDF/LorenzNewman2002_Personality.pdf
We see similar findings with IQ. An IQ of 75 is usually associated with a wide range of mental and behavioral deficits in Euro American subjects. In contrast, an African American with an IQ of 75 usually seems fairly normal.
- I'm sure there are plenty of societies that encourage individualism and have low kinship ties.
I wish I could be as sure as you. Please educate me.
- In fact, most cultures have pretty much a similar moral code.
You need to get out more.
- East Asian cultures have the best of both, a great moral code and respect for kin.
East Asian cultures have followed a path of cultural evolution that is both different from and similar to our own. It's similar in the sense they have met the challenge of developing large, complex, orderly, and relatively peaceful societies. They have done this with a somewhat different package of mental and behavioral traits, some of which are softwired and others hardwired. I've written on this subject at:
http://www.unz.com/pfrost/two-paths/
- The most recent genetic research on heritability suggests that psychopathy has a higher level then 'mild'.
I agree. When we measure the heritability of mental traits in twin studies, we end up underestimating because of errors in measurement (understanding of the question, transcription errors, unconscious differences in methodology, etc.). Such errors inflate "environmental" variability, as do spontaneous genetic mutations and developmental errors in utero.
Anonymous,
Please cite sources elaborating on East Asians' "moral code". What has been observed and reported in Japan's society is the tendency to avoid conflict and the immense pressure to remain within the bounds of what is deems acceptable. Japan has a shame oriented culture, rather than a guilt oriented one.
"As group membership in Japanese society does not come easily and individual identity is often defined by the group or groups to which one belongs, a show of indifference from other members of one’s group is a powerful incentive to stop whatever you are doing and reestablish communication with other members of your group. As obtaining acceptance in a Japanese group is primarily a matter of learning what is and what is not acceptable behavior, most members never need to be told what is correct and incorrect behavior, because everyone knows to follow those who came before them.
[...]What makes Japan a high trust society in the Fukuyama sense is then patterned way of organizing things-- not a deeply rooted psychological predisposition of generosity and comradeship towards complete strangers." (Stegeman, 2010)
sources: http://www.hashimori.com/moogoonghwa/Imagine/jway/jikochuu.html#top,
http://www.hashimori.com/moogoonghwa/Imagine/imagine.pdf
"[…]in more collectivistic cultures, the expression of emotions is practiced with concern for negatively affecting social relations, whereas in more individualistic cultures, the expression of thoughts and feelings is valued as a sign of an independent self (Kim et al., 2011)"
source:http://evoandproud.blogspot.com/2013/02/facial-expressions-in-manga-japanese.html
Post a Comment