Bronislaw Malinowski with natives on the Trobriand
Islands (1918 - source). Pro-social behavior seems to be a human universal, but is the
same true for full empathy?
What is empathy? It has at least three components:
- pro-social behavior, i.e., actions of compassion
to help others
- cognitive empathy, i.e., capacity to understand
another person's mental state
- affective or emotional empathy, i.e., capacity to
respond with the appropriate emotion to another person's mental state (Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen, 2013)
In their review of the literature, Chakrabarti and
Baron-Cohen (2013) conclude that all three components are moderately to highly
heritable, although the affective component seems to show the highest
heritability (68%). This is in line with Davis et al. (1994), who found
significant heritability for the affective facets of empathy (empathic concern
and personal distress) but not for non-affective perspective taking.
All three components can vary from one individual to
another, although studies to date have focused on pathological variation:
For example, it is suggested that people with psychopathic personality disorder may have intact cognitive empathy (hence being able to deceive others), but impaired affective empathy (hence being able to hurt others), whilst people with autism may show the opposite profile (hence finding the social world confusing because of their deficit in cognitive empathy, but not being over-represented among criminal offenders, having no wish to hurt others, suggesting their affective empathy may be intact) (Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen, 2013)
People with depression may suffer from too much
empathy, i.e., being too sensitive to the needs or distress of others (O'Connor et al., 2007). In short, these disorders seem to be the tail ends of a normal
distribution. By focusing on these extremes, we forget that most of the genetic
variability in empathy occurs among healthy individuals (Gillberg, 2007).
Using research findings on autism and Asperger syndrome,
Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen (2013) have identified nine candidate genes that
seem to promote empathy. They fall into three functional categories:
sex-steroid synthesis and metabolism; neural development and connectivity; and
social-emotional responsivity. The first category includes the degree to which
a fetus is androgenized or estrogenized before birth, as shown by digit ratio
(Frost, 2014).
Variation among
human populations
If the genes associated with empathy vary among
healthy individuals, do they also vary among human populations? This would be
expected because populations have differed in their needs for different
components of empathy, particularly since hunting and gathering gave way to
farming some 10,000 years ago—when genetic change speeded up over a
hundred-fold. At that time, humans were no longer adapting to new physical
environments. They were adapting to new cultural environments that differed in
social structure, in division of labor, in means of subsistence, in norms of
conduct, in future time orientation, in degree of sedentary living, and so on.
Our ancestors were now reshaping their environments, and these human-made
environments were now reshaping them—in other words, gene-culture co-evolution
(Hawks et al., 2007).
Humans have been transformed especially by the shift
from small bands of hunter-gatherers to larger and more complex groups of
farmers and townsfolk. With social relations expanding beyond the circle of
close kin, kinship obligations were no longer enough to ensure mutual
assistance and stop free riding. There was thus selection for pro-social
behavior, i.e., a spontaneous willingness to help not only kin but also
non-kin.
Pro-social behavior is attested across a wide range
of cultures. It is the subject of a recent book about the nature and limits of
empathy in Oceanic cultures. The Banabans of Fiji for instance express the idea
of pro-sociality through the term nanoanga,
which they normally translate into English by "compassion" or
"pity."
[...] compassion is the basis for their capacity to bond socially with others, even compassion to the point of readiness to take strangers into their community. Their empathy therefore relates causally to how they act socially toward others. Here compassion or pity embraces both understanding and fellow feeling: the islanders understand that the stranded mariner is at the end of his strength, which is why they succor him and treat him as one of their own. They understand him because he, like them, is a human being, a person. [...] Thus, for example, when someone passing by a house does not belong to the immediate family of those inside, it is customary to welcome the passer-by by calling out the words mai rin! (Come in!), which carry the implication that food and drink will not be found wanting inside. (Hermann, 2011, p. 31)
This desire to help non-kin is not unconditional.
The author notes that prior experiences with an individual in distress can
determine whether compassion will be given or withheld. Moreover, Barnabans can
"proceed strategically when deciding whether to extend trust to others or
to keep thoughts and feelings to themselves" (Hermann, 2011, p. 31). This
is not the affective empathy of entering another person's mind to feel his or
her pain.
When the Barnabans compare themselves with others, and when by their behavior toward the stranger they show that they understand him and feel with him, they do not, however, equate themselves fully and entirely with him. (Hermann, 2011, p. 32)
Another contributor to the same book writes
similarly about the inhabitants of Vanatinai, in the Trobriand Islands.
On the island of Vanatinai, when someone, including an ethnographer, privately asks a trusted confidant, "Why did she/he act like that?" "What was she/he thinking?" the common answer, often uttered in tones of puzzlement and despair, or anxiety and fear, expresses one of the islanders' core epistemological principles: "We cannot know their renuanga." Renuanga is a word that refers to a person's inner experiences, both and inseparably thought and emotion.
[...] And their psychic states, their inner thought and feelings, are inherently unknowable. It may never be clear why they were angry or sympathetic, and what caused them to act and influence an event in someone's life [...] (Lepowsky, 2011, p. 44)
In short, Oceanic cultures display hospitality but
not full empathy, which would be considered undesirable anyway:
The philosophical principle of personal opacity, the interiority of others' thoughts/feelings (renuanga), is closely bound to the islanders' fierce insistence on personal autonomy, both as cultural ideology and as daily social practice (Lepowsky, 2011, p. 47)
From
pro-sociality to full empathy
Whereas pro-sociality is attested across a wide
range of cultures, full cognitive/affective empathy is more localized. The
difference is like the one we see between shame and guilt. Most cultures
primarily use shame to enforce correct behavior, i.e., if other people see you
breaking a rule, you feel ashamed and this feeling is reinforced by social
disapproval. In contrast, only a minority of cultures—largely those of
Northwest Europe—rely primarily on guilt, which operates even when only you see
yourself breaking a rule or merely think about breaking a rule (Benedict, 1946;
Creighton, 1990).
Northwest Europeans have thus undergone two parallel
changes in behavioral control: 1) a shift from pro-sociality to full
cognitive/affective empathy; and 2) a shift from shame to guilt. Indeed, full
empathy and guilt may be two sides of the same coin. Both are the consequences
of a mental model that is used to simulate how another person thinks or feels
(an imaginary witness to a wrongful act, a person in distress) and to ensure
correct behavior by inducing the appropriate feelings (anguish, pity).
Finally, full empathy and guilt are most adaptive
where kinship ties are relatively weak and where rules of correct behavior
require a leveling of the playing field between kin and non-kin. This has long
been the case in Northwest Europe. There seems to be a longstanding pattern of
weak kinship ties west of a line running from Trieste to St. Petersburg, as
shown by several culture traits that are rare or absent elsewhere:
- relatively late marriage for men and women
- many people who never marry
- neolocality (children leave the family household
to form new households)
- high circulation of non-kin among different
households (typically young people sent out as servants) (Hajnal, 1965)
Commonly called the Western European Marriage
Pattern, this geographic zone of relatively weak kinship was thought to have
arisen after the Black Death of the 14th century. There is now good evidence
for its existence before the Black Death and fragmentary evidence going back to
9th century France and even earlier (Hallam, 1985; Seccombe, 1992, p. 94).
Historian Alan Macfarlane likewise sees an English tendency toward weaker
kinship ties before the 13th century and even during Anglo-Saxon times
(Macfarlane, 2012; Macfarlane, 1992, pp. 173-174).
This weak kinship zone may have arisen in prehistory
along the coasts of the North Sea and the Baltic, which were once home to a
unique Mesolithic culture (Price, 1991). An abundance of marine resources
enabled hunter-fisher-gatherers to achieve high population densities by
congregating each year in large coastal agglomerations for fishing, sealing,
and shellfish collecting. Population densities were comparable in fact to those
of farming societies, but unlike the latter there was much "churning"
because these agglomerations formed and reformed on a yearly basis. Kinship
obligations would have been insufficient to resolve disputes peaceably, to
manage shared resources, and to ensure respect for social rules. Initially,
peer pressure was probably used to get people to see things from the other
person's perspective. Over time, however, the pressure of natural selection
would have favored individuals who more readily felt this equivalence of
perspectives, the result being a progressive hardwiring of compassion and shame
and their gradual transformation into empathy and guilt (Frost, 2013a; Frost, 2013b).
Empathy and guilt are brutally effective ways to
enforce social rules. If one disobeys these internal overseers, the result is
self-punishment that passes through three stages: anguish, depression and,
ultimately, suicidal ideation.
People suffering from depression are looking at both others and themselves with suspicion, often believing whatever they have was obtained by cheating, and that it is more than they deserve. Depressives, burdened by moralistic standards, are harsh evaluators of both themselves and others. The self-punishment meted out by depressives is a common if disturbing symptom; while thinking 'I deserve this', they may engage in altruistic punishment turned upon the self. Just as altruistic punishers experience a neuronally based reward from punishing defectors, despite material costs, depressed patients report a sense of relief upon inflicting self-punishment. Patients who are 'cutters', describe relief from tension after cutting and depressives with suicidal ideation may describe the relief they felt when on the verge of attempting a suicidal action. (O'Connor et al., 2007, p. 67)
This pathology is progressively less common in
populations farther south and east, not so much because each stage is less
common but rather because depression is much less likely to result from
empathic guilt and much less likely to lead to suicide (Stompe et al., 2001).
This 3-stage sequence does not seem to be a human universal, at least not to
the same extent as in Northwest Europeans, a reality that Frantz Fanon noted
when describing clinical depression in Algerians:
French psychiatrists in Algeria found themselves faced with a difficult problem. When treating a melancholic patient, they were accustomed to being afraid of suicide. The melancholic Algerian kills, however. This disease of the moral conscience that is always accompanied by self-accusation and self-destructive tendencies assumes hetero-destructive forms in the Algerian. The melancholic Algerian does not commit suicide. He kills. (Fanon, 1970, pp. 219-220)
References
Benedict, R. (1946 [2005]). The Chrysanthemum and the Sword. Patterns of Japanese Culture,
First Mariner Books.
Chakrabarti, B. and S. Baron-Cohen. (2013).
Understanding the genetics of empathy and the autistic spectrum, in S.
Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, M. Lombardo. (eds). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Social
Neuroscience, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
http://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=eTdLAAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA326&ots=fHpygaxaMQ&sig=_sJsVgdoe0hc-fFbzaW3GMEslZU#v=onepage&q&f=false
Creighton, M.R. (1990). Revisiting shame and guilt
cultures: A forty-year pilgrimage, Ethos,
18, 279-307.
http://sfprg.org/control_mastery/docs/revisitshameguilt.pdf
Davis, M.H., C. Luce, and S.J. Kraus. (1994). The
heritability of characteristics associated with dispositional empathy, Journal of Personality, 62, 369-391.
Fanon, F. (1970). Les damnés de la terre, Paris: Maspero.
Frost, P. (2013a). The origins of Northwest European
guilt culture, Evo and Proud,
December 7
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2013/12/the-origins-of-northwest-european-guilt.html
Frost, P. (2013b). Origins of Northwest European
guilt culture, Part II, Evo and Proud,
December 14
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2013/12/origins-of-northwest-european-guilt.html
Frost, P. (2014). A pathway to pro-social behavior, Evo and Proud, May 10.
http://evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2014/05/a-pathway-to-pro-social-behavior.html
Gillberg, C. (2007). Non-autism childhood
personality disorders, in: T.F.D. Farrow and P.W.R. Woodruff (eds). Empathy in Mental Illness, (pp.
111-125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hajnal, J. (1965). European marriage pattern in
historical perspective. In D.V. Glass and D.E.C. Eversley (eds). Population in History, Arnold, London.
Hallam, H.E. (1985). Age at first marriage and age
at death in the Lincolnshire Fenland, 1252-1478, Population Studies, 39,
55-69.
Hawks, J., E.T. Wang, G.M. Cochran, H.C. Harpending,
& R.K. Moyzis. (2007). Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences (USA), 104, 20753-20758.
http://harpending.humanevo.utah.edu/Documents/accel_pnas_submit.pdf
Hermann, E. (2011). Empathy, ethnicity, and the self
among the Barnabans in Fiji, in D.W. Hollan, C. J. Throop (eds).The Anthropology of Empathy: Experiencing
the Lives of Others in Pacific Societies, (pp. 25-42), New York: Berghahn.
Lepowsky, M. (2011). The boundaries of personhood,
the problem of empathy, and "the native's point of view" in the outer
islands, in D.W. Hollan, C. J. Throop (eds).The
Anthropology of Empathy: Experiencing the Lives of Others in Pacific Societies,
(pp. 43-68), New York: Berghahn.
Macfarlane, A. (1992). On individualism, Proceedings of the British Academy, 82, 171-199.
http://www.alanmacfarlane.com/TEXTS/On_Individualism.pdf
Macfarlane, A. (2012). The invention of the modern
world. Chapter 8: Family, friendship and population, The Fortnightly Review, Spring-Summer serial
http://fortnightlyreview.co.uk/2012/07/invention-8/
O'Connor, L.E., J.W. Berry, T. Lewis, K. Mulherin,
and P.S. Crisostomo. (2007). Empathy and depression: the moral system in
overdrive, in: T.F.D. Farrow and P.W.R. Woodruff (eds). Empathy in Mental Illness, (pp. 49-75). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
http://www.eparg.org/publications/empathy-chapter-web.pdf
Price, T.D. (1991). The Mesolithic of Northern
Europe, Annual Review of Anthropology,
20, 211-233.
http://www.cas.umt.edu/departments/anthropology/courses/anth254/documents/annurev.an.TDouglasPrice1991MseolithicNEurope.pdf
Seccombe, W. (1992). A Millennium of Family Change. Feudalism to Capitalism in Northwestern
Europe, London: Verso.
Stompe, T., G. Ortwein-Swoboda, H.R. Chaudhry, A.
Friedmann, T. Wenzel, and H. Schanda. (2001). Guilt and depression: a
cross-cultural comparative study, Psychopathology,
34, 289-298.