The Starry Night, Vincent
van Gogh (1853-1890).
The more you empathize with the world, the more you feel its joy and pain, but
too much can lead to overload.
One
of my interests is affective empathy, the involuntary desire not only to
understand another person's emotional state but also to make it one's own—in
short, to feel the pain and joy of other people. This mental trait has a
heritability of 68% and is normally distributed along a bell curve within any
one population (Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen, 2013). Does it also vary
statistically among human populations? This is possible. Different cultures
give varying importance to affective empathy, and humans have adapted much more
to their cultural environments than to their natural environments. This is why
human genetic evolution accelerated over 100-fold about 10,000 years ago when
humans began to abandon hunting and gathering for farming, which in turn led to
increasingly diverse forms of social organization (Hawks et al., 2007).
I
have argued previously that Europeans to the north and west of the Hajnal Line
(an imaginary line running from Trieste to Saint-Petersburg) have adapted to a
cultural environment of weaker kinship and, conversely, greater individualism. In
such an environment, the reciprocal obligations of kinship are insufficient to
ensure compliance with social rules. This isn’t a new situation. Weak kinship is
inherent to the Western European Marriage Pattern, which goes back to at least
the 12th century, if not earlier.
This
cultural environment has selected for a package of mental adaptations:
-
capacity to internalize punishment for disobedience of social rules (guilt
proneness)
-
capacity to simulate and then transfer to oneself the emotional states of
people who may be affected by rule-breaking (affective empathy)
-
desire to seek out and expel rule-breakers from the moral community
(ideological intolerance).
The
above mental package has enabled Northwest Europeans to free themselves from
the limitations of kinship and organize their societies along other lines,
notably the market economy, the modern State, and political ideology. They have
thus managed to meet the threefold challenge of creating larger societies,
ensuring greater compliance with social rules, and making possible a higher level of
personal autonomy.
So
much for the theory. What direct evidence do we have that affective empathy is
stronger on average in Northwest Europeans? We know that a higher capacity for
affective empathy is associated with a larger amygdala, which seems to control our
response to facial expressions of fear and other signs of emotional distress
(Marsh et al., 2014). Two studies, one American and one English, have found
that "conservatives" tend to have a larger right amygdala (Kanai et al., 2011; Schreiber et al., 2013). In both cases, my hunch is that
"conservatives" are disproportionately drawn from populations that
have, on average, a higher capacity for affective empathy.
But
testing this kind of hunch would require a large-scale comparative study, which
in turn would require cutting up a lot of cadavers or doing a lot of MRIs. It
would be nicer to have a genetic marker that shows up on a simple test. It
would also be cheaper.
We
may now have that marker: a deletion variant of the ADRA2b gene. Carriers remember emotionally arousing images more
vividly and for a longer time, and they also show more activation of the
amygdala when viewing such images (Todd and Anderson, 2009; Todd et al., 2015).
This is not to say that the ADRA2b
deletion variant is the sole reason or even the major reason why some people
have increased capacity for affective empathy. As with intelligence, an increase
in capacity seems to have come about through changes of small effect at many
genes.
Nor
can we say that "emotional memory" is equivalent to affective
empathy. Instead, it seems to be one component, albeit a critical one: the
capacity to imagine an emotional state based on visual information (a picture
of a person's face, a puppy dog, etc.) and then keep it as part of one's
current emotional experience. Emotional memory may be upstream to affective empathy,
being perhaps closer to cognitive empathy—the ability to imagine how another
person feels without involuntarily making that feeling one's own.
Does its incidence
differ among human populations?
This
variant was first studied in the United States. Small et al. (2001) found a
higher incidence in Caucasians (31%) than in African Americans (12%). Belfer et al. (2005) likewise found a higher incidence in Caucasians (37%) than in
African Americans (21%).
In
a press release, the authors of the latest study noted that this variant is not
equally common in all humans:
The
ADRA2b deletion variant appears in
varying degrees across different ethnicities. Although roughly 50 per cent of
the Caucasian population studied by these researchers in Canada carry the
genetic variation, it has been found to be prevalent in other ethnicities. For
example, one study found that just 10 per cent of Rwandans carried the ADRA2b gene variant. (UBC News, 2015)
Curiously,
its incidence seems higher among “Canadian Caucasians” (50%) than among
"American Caucasians” (31-37%). This may reflect differences in
participant recruitment or in ethnic mix between the two countries. Indeed, the
"Caucasian" category may prove to be problematic because it includes
people from both sides of the Hajnal Line. If the average incidence is 31% to
50%, there may be populations that score much higher.
I
have found only three studies on specific European ethnicities. The first study
found an incidence of 50% in Swiss participants (de Quervain, 2007). The second
one found an incidence of 56% in Dutch participants (Cousijn et al., 2010). The
third one had two groups of participants: Israeli Holocaust survivors and a
control group of European-born Israelis who had emigrated with their parents to
the British Mandate of Palestine. The incidence was 48% in the Holocaust
survivors and 63% in the controls (Fridman et al., 2012).
From
East Asia, a study on Chinese participants reported an incidence of 68% (Zhang et al., 2005). This is surprising because Chinese seem less likely to
distinguish between cognitive empathy and affective empathy (Siu and Shek,2005). Japanese participants had an incidence of 56% in one study (Suzuki et al., 2003) and 71% in another (Ishii et al., 2015). Among the Shors, a Turkic
people of Siberia, the incidence was 73%. Curiously, the incidence was higher
in men (79%) than in women (69%). It may be that male non-carriers had a higher
death rate, since the incidence increased with age (Mulerova et al., 2015).
Conclusion
The
picture is still incomplete but the incidence of the ADRA2b deletion variant seems to range from a low of 10% in some
sub-Saharan African groups to a high of 50-65% in some European groups and
55-75% in some East Asian groups. Given the high values for East Asians, I
suspect this variant is not a marker for affective empathy per se but rather
for empathy in general (cognitive and affective).
It
may be significant that a high incidence was found among the Shors, who were
largely hunter-gatherers until recent times. This suggests that empathy reached
high levels in Eurasia long before the advent of complex societies, or even
farming. The example of the Shors also suggests that non-carriers of the
deletion variant suffer from higher mortality—a somewhat surprising finding,
given the evidence that carriers have a higher risk of heart disease.
More
research is needed on how this variant interacts with variants at other genes.
For instance, it has been found that people with at least one copy of the short
allele of 5-HTTLPR tend to be too
sensitive to negative emotional information. This effect seems to be attenuated
by the deletion variant of ADRA2b,
which either keeps one from dwelling too much on a bad emotional experience or
helps one anticipate and prevent repeat experiences (Naudts et al., 2012).
Nonetheless, too much affective empathy may lead to an overload where one ends
up helping others to the detriment of oneself and one’s family and kin.
References
Belfer,
I., B. Buzas, H. Hipp, G. Phillips, J. Taubman, I. Lorincz, C. Evans, R.H.
Lipsky, M.-A. Enoch, M.B. Max, and D. Goldman. (2005). Haplotype-based analysis
of alpha 2A, 2B, and 2C adrenergic receptor genes captures information on
common functional loci at each gene. Journal
of Human Genetics, 50, 12-20.
http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Mary_Anne_Enoch/publication/8134892_Haplotype-based_analysis_of_alpha_2A_2B_and_2C_adrenergic_receptor_genes_captures_information_on_common_functional_loci_at_each_gene/links/02e7e53559c67a2c02000000.pdf
Chakrabarti,
B. and S. Baron-Cohen. (2013). Understanding the genetics of empathy and the
autistic spectrum, in S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, M. Lombardo. (eds). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from
Developmental Social Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
http://books.google.ca/books?hl=fr&lr=&id=eTdLAAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA326&ots=fHpygaxaMQ&sig=_sJsVgdoe0hc-fFbzaW3GMEslZU#v=onepage&q&f=false
Cousijn,
H., M. Rijpkema, S. Qin, H.J.F. van Marle, B. Franke, E.J. Herman, G. van
Wingen, and G. Fernández. (2010). Acute stress modulates genotype effects on
amygdala processing in humans. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 107, 9867-9872.
http://www.pnas.org/content/107/21/9867.full.pdf
de Quervain, D.J. F., I.-T.
Kolassa, V. Ertl, L.P. Onyut, F. Neuner, T. Elbert, and A. Papassotiropoulos. (2007). A deletion
variant of the alpha2b-adrenoceptor is related to emotional memory in Europeans
and Africans. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1137-1139.
Fridman,
A., M.H. van IJzendoorn, A. Sagi-Schwartz, and M.J. Bakermans-Kranenburg.
(2012). Genetic moderation of cortisol secretion in Holocaust survivors: A
pilot study on the role of ADRA2B. International
Journal of Behavioral Development. 36,
79
http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Abraham_Sagi-Schwartz/publication/230887396_Genetic_moderation_of_cortisol_secretion_in_Holocaust_survivors__A_pilot_study_on_the_role_of_ADRA2B/links/0912f505c75bb4a01d000000.pdf
Hawks,
J., E.T. Wang, G.M. Cochran, H.C. Harpending, and R.K. Moyzis. (2007). Recent
acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 104, 20753-20758.
http://harpending.humanevo.utah.edu/Documents/accel_pnas_submit.pdf
Ishii,
M., H. Katoh, T. Kurihara, and S. Shimizu. (2015). Catechol-O-methyl
transferase gene polymorphisms in Japanese patients with medication overuse
headaches. JSM Genetics and Genomics,
2(1), 1-4.
http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Masakazu_Ishii/publication/273587694_Catechol-O-methyl_transferase_gene_polymorphisms_in_Japanese_patients_with_medication_overuse_headaches/links/5507e87a0cf26ff55f7f719d.pdf
Kanai,
R., T. Feilden, C. Firth, and G. Rees. (2011). Political orientations are
correlated with brain structure in young adults. Current Biology, 21, 677
- 680.
http://www.cell.com/current-biology/abstract/S0960-9822(11)00289-2
Marsh,
A.A., S.A. Stoycos, K.M. Brethel-Haurwitz, P. Robinson, J.W. VanMeter, and E.M.
Cardinale. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary
altruists. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 111,
15036-15041.
http://www.pnas.org/content/111/42/15036.short
Mulerova,
T.A., A.Y. Yankin, Y.V. Rubtsova, A.A. Kuzmina, P.S. Orlov, N.P. Tatarnikova,
V.N. Maksimov, M.I. Voevoda, and M.Y. Ogarkov. (2015). Association of ADRA2B
polymorphism with risk factors for cardiovascular diseases in native population
of mountain Shoria. Bulletin of Siberian
Medicine, 14, 29-34.
Naudts,
K.H., R.T. Azevedo, A.S. David, K. van Heeringen, and A.A. Gibbs. (2012).
Epistasis between 5-HTTLPR and ADRA2B polymorphisms influences attentional bias
for emotional information in healthy volunteers. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 15, 1027-1036.
Schreiber,
D., Fonzo, G., Simmons, A.N., Dawes, C.T., Flagan, T., et al. (2013). Red
Brain, Blue Brain: Evaluative Processes Differ in Democrats and Republicans. PLoS ONE, 8(2): e52970.
http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0052970
Siu,
A.M.H. and D.T. L. Shek. (2005). Validation of the Interpersonal Reactivity
Index in a Chinese Context. Research on
Social Work Practice, 15,
118-126.
http://rsw.sagepub.com/content/15/2/118.short
Small,
K.M., and S.B. Liggett. (2001) Identification and functional characterization
of alpha(2)-adrenoceptor polymorphisms. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 22,
471-477.
Suzuki N, Matsunaga T,
Nagasumi K, Yamamura T, Shihara N, Moritani T, et al. (2003). a2B adrenergic receptor deletion polymorphism
associates with autonomic nervous system activity in young healthy Japanese. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Metabolism, 88, 1184-1187.
http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Tetsuro_Matsunaga/publication/10863380_Alpha(2B)-adrenergic_receptor_deletion_polymorphism_associates_with_autonomic_nervous_system_activity_in_young_healthy_Japanese/links/00b7d530379a0f06bf000000.pdf
Todd,
R.M. and A.K. Anderson. (2009). The neurogenetics of remembering emotions past.
Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences U.S.A., 106, 18881-18882
http://www.pnas.org/content/106/45/18881.short
Todd,
R.M., M.R. Ehlers, D. J. Muller, A.
Robertson, D.J. Palombo, N. Freeman, B. Levine, and A.K. Anderson (2015).
Neurogenetic Variations in norepinephrine availability enhance perceptual
vividness. The Journal of Neuroscience,
35, 6506-6516.
UBC
News. (2015). How your brain reacts to emotional information is influenced by
your genes, May 6
http://news.ubc.ca/2015/05/06/how-your-brain-reacts-to-emotional-information-is-influenced-by-your-genes/
Zhang,
H., X. Li, J. Huang, Y. Li, L. Thijs, Z. Wang, X. Lu, K. Cao, S. Xie, J.A.
Staessen, J-G. Wang. (2005). Cardiovascular and metabolic phenotypes in
relation to the ADRA2B insertion/deletion polymorphism in a Chinese population.
Journal of Hypertension, 23, 2201-2207.
http://www.staessen.net/publications/2001-2005/05-30-P.pdf
3 comments:
There's a significant difference between American whites and Canadian whites. As you probably know American whites are more likely to hold extremist or right-wing views... and to vote for Donald Trump. "We'll send drones to kill all the illegal immigrants" and "Mexicans are rapists and murderers" is the rhetoric emanating from American whites, such as Donald Trump and his followers. I doubt that these whites are overburdened with too much empathy, to be honest.
BTW, since you've written a lot on IQ in whites vs. other races, could you explain why white Americans (a supposedly high-IQ population) love Trump and are prepared to vote for him, a low-IQ man who can't form a coherent sentence, talks like a 3rd-grader according to analysts, and only knows words like "stupid" and "bad" ? He leads by double digits in the mostly-white Republican party. Even Jeb Bush, a far higher-IQ man, isn't faring too well with white Republicans. But I thought the white population had a high IQ, was intelligent, and would have preferred someone on their level.
Reader,
He's the only candidate addressing the immigration issue.
Heh. Expert neg.
Post a Comment