Anti-UKIP
protest in Edinburgh (source: Brian McNeil, Wikicommons).
"Conservative" increasingly means pro-white.
Are
liberals and conservatives differently wired? It would seem so. When brain MRIs
were done on 90 young adults from University College London, it was found that
self-described liberals tended to have more grey matter in the anterior
cingulate cortex, whereas self-described conservatives tended to have a larger
right amygdala. These results were replicated in a second sample of young adults
(Kanai et al., 2011).
The
amygdala is used to recognize fearful facial expressions, whereas the anterior
cingulate cortex serves to monitor uncertainty and conflict (Adolphs et al., 1995; Botvinick et al., 1999; Critchley et al., 2001; Kennerley et al., 2006).
Perhaps unsurprisingly, these findings were changed somewhat in the popular
press. "Conservatives Big on Fear, Brain Study Finds," ran a headline
in Psychology Today. The same article
assured its readers that the anterior cingulate cortex "helps people cope
with complexity" (Barber, 2011).
A
study on 82 young American adults came to a similar conclusion. Republicans
showed more activity in the right amygdala, and Democrats more activity in the
left insula. Unlike the English study, the anterior cingulate cortex didn't
differ between the two groups (Schreiber et al., 2013).
It
would seem, then, that conservatives and liberals are neurologically different.
Perhaps certain political beliefs will alter your mental makeup. Or perhaps
your mental makeup will lead you to certain political beliefs. But how can that
be when conservatism and liberalism have changed so much in recent times, not
only ideologically but also electorate-wise? A century ago, English
"conservatives" came from the upper class, the middle class, and
outlying rural areas. Today, Britain's leading "conservative" party,
the UKIP, is drawing more and more of its members from the urban working class—the
sort of folks who routinely voted Labour not so long ago. Similar changes have
taken place in the U.S. Until the 1950s, white southerners were overwhelmingly
Democrats. Now, they're overwhelmingly Republicans.
Of
course, the above studies are only a few years old. When we use terms like
"conservative" and "liberal" we refer to what they mean
today. Increasingly, both terms have an implicitly ethnic meaning. The UKIP is
becoming the native British party, in opposition to a growing Afro-Asian
population that votes en bloc for
Labour. Meanwhile, the Republicans are becoming the party of White Americans,
particularly old-stock ones, in opposition to a Democrat coalition of African,
Hispanic, and Asian Americans, plus a dwindling core of ethnic whites.
So
are these brain differences really ethnic differences? Neither study touches
the question. The English study assures us that the participants were
homogeneous:
We
deliberately used a homogenous sample of the UCL student population to minimize
differences in social and educational environment. The UK Higher Education
Statistics Agency reports that 21.1% of UCL students come from a working-class
background. This rate is relatively low compared to the national average of
34.8%. This suggests that the UCL students from which we recruited our
participants disproportionately have a middle-class to upper-class background.
(Kanai et al., 2011)
Yes,
the students were largely middle-class, but how did they break down ethnically?
Wikipedia provides a partial answer:
In
2013/14, 12,330 UCL students were from outside the UK (43% of the total number
of students in that year), of whom 5,504 were from Asia, 3,679 from the
European Union ex. the United Kingdom, 1,195 from North America, 516 from the
Middle East, 398 from Africa, 254 from Central and South America, and 166 from
Australasia (University College London, 2014)
These
figures were for citizenship only. We should remember that many of the UK
students would have been of non-European origin.
We
know more about the participants in the American study. They came from the
University of California, San Diego, whose student body at the time was 44%
Asian, 26% Caucasian, 10% Mexican American, 10% unknown, 4% Filipino, 3%
Latino/Other Spanish, and 2% African American (Anon, 2010). This ethnic
breakdown mirrors the party breakdown of the participants: 60 Democrats (72.5%)
and 22 Republicans (27.5%).
Affective empathy
and ethnicity
In
my last post, I cited a study showing that the amygdala is larger in
extraordinary altruists—people who have donated one of their kidneys to a
stranger. In that study, we were told that a larger amygdala is associated with
greater responsiveness to fearful facial expressions, i.e., a greater
willingness to help people in distress. Conversely, psychopaths have a smaller
amygdala and are less responsive to fearful faces (Marsh et al., 2014).
Hmm
... That's a tad different from the spin in Psychology
Today. Are liberals the ones who don't care about others? Are they ...
psychopaths?
It
would be more accurate to say that "liberals" come from populations
whose capacity for affective empathy is lower on average and who tend to view
any stranger as a potential enemy. That's most people in this world, and that's
how most of the world works. I suspect the greater ability to monitor
uncertainty and conflict reflects adaptation to an environment that has long
been socially fragmented into clans, castes, religions, etc. This may explain
why a larger anterior cingulate cortex correlated with "liberalism"
in the British study (high proportion of South Asian students) but not in the American
study (high proportion of East Asian students).
As
for "conservatives," they largely come from Northwest Europe, where a
greater capacity for affective empathy seems to reflect an environment of
relatively high individualism, relatively weak kinship, and relatively frequent
interactions with nonkin. This environment has prevailed west of the Hajnal
Line since at least the 12th century, as shown by the longstanding
characteristics of the Western European Marriage Pattern: late age of marriage
for both sexes; high rate of celibacy; strong tendency of children to form new
households; and high circulation of non-kin among families. This zone of weaker
kinship, with greater reliance on internal means of behavior control, may also
explain why Northwest Europeans are more predisposed to guilt than to shame,
whereas the reverse is generally the case elsewhere in the world (Frost, 2014).
All
of this may sound counterintuitive. Doesn't the political left currently stand
for autonomy theory and individualism? Doesn't it reject traditional values
like kinship? In theory it does. The reality is a bit different, though. When
Muslims vote Labour, it's not because they want gay marriage and teaching of
gender theory in the schools. They expect something else.
The
same goes for the political right. When former Labourites vote UKIP, it's not
because they want lower taxes for the rich and offshoring of manufacturing
jobs. They expect something else. Are they being delusional? Perhaps. But,
then, are the Muslims being delusional?
Perhaps
neither group is. Perhaps both understand what politics is really about.
References
Adolphs,
R., D. Tranel, H. Damasio, and A.R. Damasio. (1995). Fear and the human
amygdala, The Journal of Neuroscience,
15, 5879-5891.
http://www.emotion.caltech.edu/papers/AdolphsTranel1995Fear.pdf
Anon
(2010). Racial breakdown of the largest California public colleges, The Huffington Post, May 4
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2010/03/04/racial-breakdown-of-the-l_n_485577.html
Barber,
N. (2011). Conservatives big on fear, study finds, Psychology Today, April 19
http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/the-human-beast/201104/conservatives-big-fear-brain-study-finds
Botvinick,
M., Nystrom, L.E., Fissell, K., Carter, C.S., and Cohen, J.D. (1999). Conflict
monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex, Nature, 402, 179-181.
Critchley,
H.D., Mathias, C.J., and Dolan, R.J. (2001). Neural activity in the human brain
relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation, Neuron, 29, 537-545.
Frost,
P. (2014). We are not equally empathic, Evo
and Proud, November 15
http://www.evoandproud.blogspot.ca/2014/11/we-are-not-equally-empathic.html
Kanai,
R., T. Feilden, C. Firth, and G. Rees. (2011). Political orientations are
correlated with brain structure in young adults, Current Biology, 21, 677
- 680.
http://www.cell.com/current-biology/abstract/S0960-9822(11)00289-2
Kennerley,
S.W., Walton, M.E., Behrens, T.E., Buckley, M.J., and Rushworth, M.F. (2006).
Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9,
940-947.
Marsh,
A.A., S.A. Stoycos, K.M. Brethel-Haurwitz, P. Robinson, J.W. VanMeter, and E.M.
Cardinale. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary
altruists, Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 111,
15036-15041.
http://www.pnas.org/content/111/42/15036.short
Schreiber,
D., Fonzo, G., Simmons, A.N., Dawes, C.T., Flagan, T., et al. (2013). Red Brain, Blue
Brain: Evaluative Processes Differ in Democrats and Republicans. PLoS ONE 8(2): e52970.
http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0052970
University
College London. (2014). Wikipedia
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/University_College_London#Student_body